Mental Health Science Meeting 2019

This was an interesting article I found on: M. Q. Mental Health

See credits below.


Registration is now open for our next Mental Health Science Meeting, in London on 7-8 February 2019.

MQ's Mental Health Science Meeting is one of the largest international scientific meetings dedicated solely to mental health science – bringing together experts from around the world to take on the biggest challenges in mental health.

Now in its 5th year, the meeting invites researchers from across disciplines to explore cutting-edge ways to understand, treat and prevent mental illness. The theme will be 'Transforming mental health throughout the life course', with symposia looking at loneliness, the intersection of physical and mental health, and self-harm and suicide.

The 2-day programme includes keynote speeches from leading experts in the field, multidisciplinary symposia, a poster session, short talks by junior researchers and a panel discussion. The event provides a unique opportunity to showcase ideas and develop collaborative research networks.

Who is it for?

Researchers and experts from all disciplines who have an interest in mental health. We welcome researchers at all stages of their career, as well as other mental health stakeholders, including clinicians, members of the public affected by mental illness, mental health funders and charity and business representatives.

Speakers include:

  • Joshua Gordon, National Institute of Mental Health (keynote)
  • Sonia Johnson, University College London (keynote)
  • Michael Ungar, Dalhousie University (keynote)
  • Jessica Agnew-Blais, King's College London
  • Louise Arseneault, King's College London
  • Bridget Callaghan, Columbia University
  • Sarah Carr, University of Birmingham
  • Bill Fulford, University of Oxford
  • Simon Gilbody, University of York
  • Luc Goossens, KU Leuven
  • Tim Kendall, NHS England
  • Rogier Kievit, University of Cambridge
  • Richard Lindner, University of Kassel
  • Clare Llewellyn, University College London
  • Timothy Matthews, King's College London
  • Becky Mars, University of Bristol
  • Philip Shaw, National Institutes of Health (NIH)
  • Jude Stansfield, Public Health England
  • Andrew Steptoe, University College London
  • Ellen Townsend, University of Nottingham
  • Rory O'Connor, University of Glasgow
  • Pamela Qualter, University of Manchester
  • Ed Whiting, Wellcome Trust

Read the agenda here.

Location: Royal College of Physicians, 11 St Andrews Place, London NW1 4LE

Registration and Prices

The registration cost includes refreshments, lunch, and a reception with drinks and canapés during the poster session. Places are limited and offered on a first-come, first-served basis. A limited number of early-bird tickets are available. Please note that early bird and standard tickets may sell out before the dates stated below.

  • Early-bird tickets – until 16 November: £170 both days, £90 one day.
  • Standard tickets – after 16 November or after early-bird sells out: £200 both days, £110 one day.
  • Submissions for posters and travel bursaries close on 16 November.
  • Registration closes on February 1st, 2019.

Details on how to register can be found here. If you have any questions, please do not hesitate to contact us at events@mqmentalhealth.org.

Poster Submission and Travel Bursaries

Through our poster session at the meeting we showcase work from student and early-career researchers. Selected applicants will have the opportunity to present their work in the form of a short oral presentation. Applications for short talks will be reviewed on the basis of the abstract submitted.

This year we have also introduced the ability for a small number of researchers to submit Preregistration Posters at the MHSM. Preregistration posters give the opportunity to submit plans for studies yet to be undertaken, rather than work that has already been completed. During the submission process you will be given the choice of submitting a 'Preregistration poster' or a 'Standard poster'.

The main poster session will take place on Thursday 7 February 2019 and posters will be exhibited over both days of the conference. Poster applicants have one week to confirm their attendance once notified of their application status.

A limited number of travel bursaries are available for postgraduate students or early career scientists presenting their work. If you would like to be considered for a travel bursary, please submit your abstract through the registration form before November 16th.

If your application for a bursary is not successful, and you would no longer like to attend the conference, you have two weeks from being informed of this outcome to apply for a full refund of the registration cost.

Assisted Places for Service Users and Carers

A limited number of assisted places are available for people affected by mental health problems who are unwaged, self-employed or on low incomes. This includes free registration and travel costs in line with MQ’s travel policies. If you are interested in applying, please email events@mqmentalhealth.org by November 16th. Successful applicants will be drawn from a ballot and notified by email.

Join our science meeting

Secure your place at our Mental Health Science Meeting 2019

RegisterOriginal Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

Trauma in childhood can affect your health

This was an interesting article I found on: Talk Health Partnership

See credits below.


In the mid 90s a team of researchers from a private healthcare company in America made a startling discovery. They asked over 17,000 people about their experiences of abuse and trauma in childhood, and followed them over 15 years. They called what they were measuring Adverse Childhood Experiences (or ACEs) and included trauma directly affecting a child, like abuse or neglect, as well as trauma affecting the environment, like witnessing domestic violence.

The people they studied were not from disadvantaged communities; they had jobs, college degrees and good healthcare. What they found changed our understanding of the impact of trauma.

For one thing, ACEs were much more common than anyone had imagined. More than two thirds of people had at least one ACE and over 10% had four or more.

Having a history of ACEs was strongly linked to the 10 leading causes of death in the US. For example, a person with four or more ACEs had two and half times the risk of getting a respiratory disease like COPD, were four times more likely to get depression and seven times more likely to be an alcoholic compared to a person with no ACEs. A person with seven or more ACEs had triple the risk of lung cancer and three and half times the risk of ischemic heart disease.

A person with six ACEs was likely to die twenty years earlier than someone with no ACEs.

The more ACEs you experienced as a child, the more likely you are to take risks with your health: smoke, drink and take illegal substances. But even when you control for lifestyle choices, the risks are still much higher the more ACEs you’ve experienced.

Scientists believe that the relationship between ACEs and health problems later in life is linked to our automatic response to danger. When you’re exposed to a threat your body prepares itself to deal with danger; known as the fight, flight or freeze response, your heart rate increases, your pupils dilate and your body is flooded with adrenalin and cortisol. After the danger has passed, your body and all the stress hormones return to their normal state. Scientists believe that repeated triggering of this threat response in childhood causes tissue damage, inflammation and wear and tear on the body. It changes immune system functioning and even determines the way the brain develops so that parts associated with decision making and problem solving are underdeveloped whilst those parts responsible for emotional regulation are overactive.

You can score your own ACEs here: Take the ACE quiz.

If you have a high score it shouldn’t feel hopeless. We’re beginning to understand more about the things that make someone resilient and there are things you can do to help.

Learn to meditate. The brain has plasticity, which means the more you use certain parts, the more they develop. Regular meditation practice can help you respond to stress without automatically triggering the fight or flight mechanisms.

Seek support to help you cope with the trauma. Confiding in a trusted friend or family member can help. Visit the NAPAC and the NSPCC for more advice and support.

Inform yourself. Watch this excellent TED talk from Nadine Burke Harris and this informative short animation from Public Health Wales. I’ve written about the link between ACEs and psoriasis on my blog copingwithpsoriasis.com.

+6

Dr Catherine OLeary

Dr Catherine OLeary

Catherine is a Consultant Clinical Psychologist who trained at the Institute of Psychiatry in London. She specialises in working with people with long term health conditions. She has had psoriasis for over thirty years.

View Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

“We need to get better at sharing our experiences of loneliness”

This was an interesting article I found on: M. Q. Mental Health

See credits below.


Loneliness is something everyone experiences at some point in their life. However, for some people, prolonged periods of feeling lonely can negatively impact their mental health.

Pam Qualter is a Professor at the University of Manchester’s Institute of Education. She studies loneliness throughout life – particularly in adolescence – and is looking to better understand its link to depression. We spoke to Pam ahead of her appearance at this year’s Mental Health Science Meeting.

Great to speak to you, Pam – can I begin by asking what got you started with studying loneliness?

Thanks for having me! I remember a class I went to at University, where the lecturer was talking about loneliness. I had experienced loneliness as a child – my family moved around a lot, so I’d establish some good friendships, but then a year later we’d have to move and I’d have to start all over again. I was a really lonely kid.

However, the lecturer was saying that it wasn’t possible to feel loneliness as a child. I was sitting there thinking, “that’s not my experience at all – am I really that strange!?” It sparked a bit of self-evaluation – I didn’t think my experience was that unusual. That spark led to a third-year project to study it further, which then led to a PhD – and it all snowballed from there.

We now know that children and adolescents actually do experience exactly the same loneliness that adults do. Loneliness is that intense feeling of disconnect, because of a mismatch between the social relationships we want and the relationships we have.

I’m not ashamed that I have that personal experience of loneliness, in fact I like it – having that experience means I really can try and understand the experiences of other people.

How common is loneliness?

It’s incredibly common – though I have some issue with the idea that there is an ‘epidemic of loneliness’, which is something we hear about in the media. It suggests that there is something pathological about feeling lonely, something abnormal – and I don’t think there is.

My colleagues and I have looked at the prevalence of loneliness in different age groups. What we have found is something like a U-shaped curve across the lifespan – where loneliness peaks in adolescence, dips down in mid-life, and then peaks again in later life.

In adolescence, we’re trying to work out who we are, and which group we fit into – which can be a very isolating experience. However, in old age, the challenge to our relationships is that we’re losing people. Maybe we’re not able to get out to socialise as often as we were, or maybe we’re losing friends and partners to ill health.

Between these two peaks, in ‘middle age’, people generally score lower on measures of loneliness. However, there may be certain people who are prone to loneliness, such as carers, the unemployed, or new parents with their first child.

You led the BBC Loneliness Experiment, the largest study on the topic ever conducted. What was the most interesting thing you found?

The BBC Loneliness Experiment was a study I conducted in 2018, in collaboration with the Radio 4's All In The Mind programme, the Wellcome Collection, and colleagues from Brunel and Exeter Universities. We carried out a survey with just under 55,000 people over the age of 16, to gather their opinions on loneliness and relationships.

What I found most surprising was the stigma that people attach to themselves if they feel lonely. People didn’t think badly of other lonely individuals at all. But people who scored high on loneliness felt very negatively about themselves, and so they would conceal it – they felt there was a real shame in telling other people.

Let me give you an example: not long after we released the results of the BBC Loneliness Experiment, I overheard a conversation on the train between two young men. They were talking about how they thought a friend of theirs was probably quite lonely – it was a very kind conversation, they obviously wanted to help. But they admitted that “there’s no way he’ll tell us”, so they didn’t feel they could reach out to him.

It’s fascinating – loneliness is such a universal experience that most of us feel at some point, and yet that doesn’t stop us feeling really ashamed when it happens to us. We need to get better at sharing our personal experiences of loneliness.

When does loneliness become a problem for our mental health?

Feeling lonely definitely impacts your wellbeing, without a doubt – it’s a horrible experience. But for mental health, the issue comes for individuals who don’t come out of loneliness – who remain lonely for months or years – and never establish the relationships they want, or don’t deal with their inner turmoil.

Most of the research around loneliness and mental illness has focussed on the link with depression. Some researchers have found links with suicidal thoughts and with anxiety, but more research is needed to pin that down.

What we know about loneliness and depression is that there seems to be a two-way relationship. We know that adolescents who feel very lonely may show more depressive symptoms – but also if you’re depressed to start with, feelings of loneliness are more likely to arise.

So, it’s not always the case that the loneliness comes first then the depressive symptoms come after, it could work the other way round. That means there’s not going to be a simple fix.

What’s needed to break this link between loneliness and mental illness?

What I’ve proposed again and again is that we need cognitive behavioural therapies (CBT), focussed not only at depression but also at loneliness.

This kind of CBT starts with getting people to challenge the assumptions and interpretations they have about social relationships. We know that people who feel lonely for a long time tend to have very distrusting beliefs about relationships. Even if they do establish what seem like close relationships, they don’t trust that person to, say, keep secrets or support them or help them – the normal things you’d expect friends to do. CBT can help to challenge those beliefs.

What’s currently being provided to tackle loneliness?

The UK Government is funding a lot of programmes around loneliness, but they’re mostly focussed on providing people with access to social groups – with the idea being that if you make a friendship, you’re not lonely anymore. I think this is too simplistic.

Although those kinds of groups will help some people, for the chronically lonely, they just aren’t going to work – they will still be very distrusting of any relationship they build. I also think the kind of ‘meet new people’ interventions would work much better if people got together over a shared interest or hobby, rather than just being lumped together because they feel lonely!

It’s also crucial that we intervene at adolescence, and not just because teenagers feel lonely a lot – if we can give young people the knowledge and the strategies to cope with loneliness, we can set them up for life. The Government has said that loneliness will be taught in schools as part of the new PSHE curriculum about relationships – I really hope this happens.

Finally, what do you hope for the future? What needs to happen next?

I really want us to be able to develop suitable interventions to combat chronic loneliness, I don’t think we’ve cracked it yet. But to do that, we need to properly understand how loneliness works, and we need better evidence on the link between loneliness and mental illness, beyond depression.

However, I can feel that the tone of the conversation around loneliness is changing. If we can take that forward, through things like MQ’s Mental Health Science Meeting, we can keep that momentum going to help people manage their loneliness, and not be ashamed to talk about it.

Pam will be speaking at this year’s Mental Health Science Meeting – an event bringing together researchers across different disciplines to explore cutting-edge new ways to understand, treat and prevent mental illness. To find out more, click here.

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

MQ Mental Health Science Meeting 2019 – Live blog

This was an interesting article I found on: M. Q. Mental Health

See credits below.


Hello and welcome to the live blog for the 2019 MQ Mental Health Science Meeting!

You can follow the meeting on Twitter and share your thoughts using the hashtag #MQScienceMeeting – follow us at @MQmentalhealth for regular updates.

You can also stay informed on the discussions right here on the live blog. This page will be updated frequently throughout the meeting, and you can jump to particular sessions or speakers using the links below:

DAY ONE

DAY TWO

DAY ONE

Introduction

Hello and welcome to the live blog for the 2019 MQ Mental Health Science Meeting!

Now in its 5th year, it’s one of the largest international scientific meetings dedicated solely to mental health science.

You’re joining researchers, healthcare professionals and members of the mental health community from around the world, to take on the biggest challenges in mental health.

Over the next two days, experts from a wide range of disciplines will be discussing their work and sharing their ideas on how we can best understand, treat and ultimately prevent mental illness.

The theme for the 2019 meeting is “Transforming mental health throughout the life course”. Many of the talks will be focussing on how our mental health – and how we might approach prevention and treatment of mental illness – changes throughout our lives.

09.30 – Welcome: Dr Sophie Dix, MQ: Transforming Mental Health

Dr Sophie Dix, Director of Research at MQ, welcomes everyone to the meeting.

Sophie sets the scene for why we're all here. We're at a tipping point of increased awareness of mental health. MQ and this meeting exist to ensure that mental health research is part of that conservation.

We need to tackle the big challenges. MQ's supporters, many of people whom have lived experience of mental illness are tired of 'trial-and-error' treatments. They're tired of delayed diagnosis. They're tired of treatments not working.

Sophie says we need to tackle the status quo. Only 2.7% of funding for mental health research comes from public donations – compared to 67% for cancer research. MQ exists to champion mental health research, to champion mental health research for the public.

Five years ago at this meeting, there were only 77 attendees – today we have 277 people registered. The Mental Health Science Meeting exists to break down silos and get people talking to one another – researchers, clinicians, and people affected by mental illness.

You can listen to an interview with Sophie below:

09.40 – Keynote 1: Sonia Johnson, “Should we prioritise social and self-management interventions in efforts to improve mental health outcomes through research?”

Our first talk of the meeting is a keynote speech from Prof Sonia Johnson from University College London. She’s calling for a change in how we conduct research and deliver mental health services.

Sonia begins by stating the progress made in tackling mental health problems – for example, in psychosis and bipolar disorder – has been disappointing. Rates of relapse have remained stubbornly static, and the 'mortality gap' (the rate of death amongst people with severe mental ilness compared to the general population) is widening, and the employment rate in people with psychosis has been going down. This is in stark contrast to cancer, for example, where on the whole, mortality rates have improved dramatically over the last few decades.

There are several factors which she believes have contribute towards this. One of these is the lack of investment, which means we're still waiting for the 'great leap forward' in treatments. Other factors include the stigma that is still attached to mental illness, and social factors which are hard for individuals to change – e.g. poverty, and other inequalities.

However, Sonia says that by listening to mental health services, we can make sure we're focussing on the 'right' type of research. The type of outcomes that people facing mental illness often say they value the most are social, not about relapse rates or symptoms. Community mental health teams are key to these kinds of outcomes, but there's little research into how they can provide the best care on a day-to-day basis.

Sonia sets out three priorities to help us do better in mental health research:

  1. It’s time to actually implement things that have been shown to work.
  2. For interventions that aren’t quite at the stage where we’re ready to implement them, we need to find ways to translate their findings into practise.
  3. We need to develop new interventions to address social problems like loneliness.

Sonia starts with an example of an intervention, 'individual placement and support' with good evidence which is 'ready to implement. Individual placement and support can help with ensure people get back into work.

The problem is the 'implementation gap' – interventions which have been shown to be effective, but which haven't yet been rolled out into everyday care for people with mental illness.

Sonia say we need to make friends with 'implementation scientists' – researchers whose job it is to understand how the barriers which prevent interventions from reaching people who need them can be overcome.

Sonia's second priority is to understand how we can scale-up the interventions we are developing now, so that they can be ready to implement. One example of this from Sonia's own research is 'supported self-management' – helping people to decide upon their own priorities for recovery and care, but with the support of others around them.

People who had recently experienced a mental health crisis were asked to fill out a ‘personal recovery workbook’, which included setting goals and plans for future crises. Participants were offered sessions with a ‘peer support worker’ – someone with their own personal experience of mental health crises – to help them in filling out the workbook. Sonia and her colleagues found that people who had a peer support worker were 34% less likely to be readmitted to acute care within one year.

Sonia says the challenge now is to try and weaving into the fabric of mental health care.

Sonia moves onto her third priority – we need to develop new social interventions, aimed at tackling social issues. These kinds of interventions are challening to develop for many reasons, but there is a strong case for focussing on this now. They can bring a fresh new approach to improve the situation for people with mental illness – both in treatment and in prevention.

An example of a social issue where we lack interventions with good evidence with which to tackle it, is loneliness – the subject of the next symposium.

Sonia shares an example of something she's been working on to tackle loneliness – the Community Navigator Study. The project is still in its early stages, but so far found, they've found that, with a group of people in four London boroughs, it's acceptable and feasible to implement.

Want to find out more about Sonia's research? Listen to an interview with her, by the Mental Elf.

10:30 – Symposium 1: "Loneliness and mental health: a two-way relationship?"

Next comes our first symposium of four at the meeting. The format of the symposium is simple – four speakers will talk on a particular theme, with time at the end devoted to further discussion, including questions from the audience.

Each symposium will have two people ‘chairing’ the session. One will be a research expert in the area, and – for the first time ever at the Mental Health Science Meeting – the other will be a person with lived experience, either directly or indirectly affected by the issue at hand.

The topic for our first symposium is loneliness – a very common experience which can shape our mental health.

Our chairs for this session are Prof Til Wykes from King’s College London, and Charly Cox, a poet and MQ ambassador living with bipolar disorder.

You can read more about Charly’s experience and her struggle to get a diagnosis in this blog.

10:35 – Pamela Qualter, “Children’s Experiences of Loneliness”

Prof Pam Qualter from the University of Manchester kicks off the symposium. Her research has looked at loneliness throughout life, but her main focus – and the subject of her talk today – is on children.

We were lucky enough to have a chance to interview Pam before the meeting, you can read more about her work in this Q&A.

Pam's first point is that social relationships matter – we need to know that we have people around us who 'have our back'. This comes out of our evolutionary history – human and other animals who formed groups for support are the most successful species.

Loneliness is not just about being alone – it is also about the threat of being alone, of feeling disconnected, of not belonging. Loneliness results from the mismatch of the kinds of relationships we want, and the kinds of relationships we have.

Throughout her career, Pam's research has focussed on study loneliness in childhood – in particular to understand whether children feel the same kind of loneliness as adults say they do. The short answer of all this research is yes – children can feel lonely, and the way they describe it is exactly the same as adults.

There's varying different relationships that people want, and which can be sources of loneliness, which change over time. The first is peer friendship – a close friend, which is particularly important to children. The second is peer group – being part of a group, not just with one person – which is particular important to teenagers. Finally, romantic relationships, which develop from adolescence onwards.

Everyone feels lonely, at various points during our lives, but there's certain times at which loneliness peaks. Loneliness is particularly common in adolescence and old age – because of the kinds of relationships we are developing, changing, and losing at these ages.

Pam wants to get across that loneliness is more of a symptom, not a disease. She says that loneliness is adaptive – it's horrible when we experience it, but it actually makes us want to make a change, to reconnect with others.

The problem with loneliness is when it is prolonged – people with chronic loneliness report multiple health problems, and as we'll see, mental illness.

Pam and her colleagues have shown that prolonged loneliness in childhood can lead to depressive symptoms in adolescence. And Pam's research shows that the relationship is two-way – depression can also increase feelings of loneliness.

Pam finishes with talking about how we might be able to intervene with childhood loneliness. Her research has found that CBT type therapies, to challenge people's beliefs about relationships (particularly distrust), are particularly effective.

One question from the audience is about social media and it's impact on loneliness. Pam shares that it depends on how teenagers use social media. If it's to interact with friends or other people, social media can be helpful – but if it's used as a distraction, it may have no impact on feeling lonely.

Listen to Pam talk about her research in a podcast recorded for The Mental Elf.

10:55 – Luc Goossens, “Loneliness in Adolescence”

The next speaker in the symposium is Prof Luc Goossens, from the University of Leuven in Belgium. Luc continues the theme of loneliness in young people, this time in adolescents.

Luc considers loneliness as a signal which we use to make a change – like hunger signals to us that we need to eat, loneliness can signal to us that we need to reconnect with others.

As we heard from Pam Qualter’s talk before, adolescence is a period of our lives when we are particularly vulnerable to loneliness. There's plenty of other times where loneliness peaks, for many reasons, though Luc's message is clear – it's not just old people who feel lonely.

There are many negative consequences of loneliness, for our physical and mental health. Some of the research from Luc's research team has shown that loneliness goes hand in hand with social anxiety and depressive symptoms.

Luc moves onto the two types of loneliness in adolescence, connected to two different types of relationships. The first is with peers – the feelings that a person has fewer friends than others, or no friends at all. The second is with parents – feeling that your parents don't understand you.

There's also differing attitudes to loneliness – some people feel bad about being alone, but for others, feeling lonely can make them feel stronger and more motivated. Research has shown that during adolescence, teenagers tend to feel less negative about loneliness, and more positive about it.

However, this is not the same for all teenagers. Luc shows that there appears to be five different groups which each have their own 'trajectory' of loneliness, how their loneliness changes over time. The most common group is 'stable low' – who generally have low levels of loneliness throughout adolescence. Other groups have increasing or descreasing levels of loneliness, and a further group – 'stable high' – feeling very lonely throughout their teenage years.

Luc's take-home message is that the challenge for adolescents is to try and avoid prolonged periods of feeling lonely, and to find constructive ways to of dealing with time spent on their own.

11:20 – Tim Matthews, “Lonely young adults in modern Britain: Findings from an epidemiological cohort study”

Next up in this symposium on loneliness is Dr Tim Matthews from King’s College London. Timothy is discussing loneliness in young adults, and in particular, talking about a study of over 2,000 young people in England & Wales.

Tim thinks that 2018 was a milestone year for loneliness. The government created a 'Minister for Loneliness', and there was a big increase in the conversation in the media around loneliness. But the key focus in this conversation was on older people. As we have already heard, loneliness is not something that only affects the eldery, it is also something which affects younger people too.

But just because, when we look at the whole population, loneliness peaks in adolescence then decreases into adulthood – it doesn't mean that we shouldn't worry about teenage loneliness, or think that it's something that everyone will 'grow out of' eventually. Some young people never 'emerge' from loneliness experienced in adolescence – we need to better understand loneliness in young adults, to understand their experiences.

Tim goes on to talk about his study of a large group of young people. He and his colleagues found that loneliness was common amongst 18-year olds – a quarter of the respondents said they feel lonely some of the time, and 7% said their felt lonely often. Loneliness did not discriminate – there's didn't seem to be any particular socio-economic group of young people who experienced loneliness more than others.

Tim shows that loneliness in this group are more likely to also have mental health problems, such as depression and anxiety. Loneliness was also connected to poor sleep quality, and engage in less physical activity.

And loneliness is connected to their general attitudes too – lonely people were less likely to be satisfied with their life, and feel unable to cope with stress. Lonely individuals were more likely to be out of work, feel less confident about their abilities, and feel less optimistic about their prospects of getting a job.

Tim's message is that loneliness affects lots of areas of our lives. Doctors should be particularly focussed on people who say feel lonely, because it could be an indicator of other problems too. Early intervention is absolutely key to reducing loneliness, as soon as possible.

In terms of interventions – 'social prescribing' is all the rage at the moment, where people are brought together in groups, creating new opportunities for lonely people to meet others. But we do need to be mindful of how effective these are. People can still feel lonely, even in social groups. A 'one-size-fits-all' approach will not work, we need to support social prescribing with more targetted interventions and counselling.

11:45 – Jude Stansfield, “A Public Mental Health approach to loneliness”

Last to speak in this symposium on loneliness is Jude Stansfield, the National Advisor on mental health to Public Health England. Her talk is focussing on tackling loneliness from a public health perspective, to improve the country’s mental health.

Jude starts by reiterating that being connected to others matters for our health – both physical and mental. Given this connection, she considers a loneliness to be as important to our nation's health as smoking or obesity.

Jude says that loneliness is a complex issue, which shouldn’t be seen as just affecting one individual, or a medical problem, but as a social issue involving many factors.

Part of the work Jude is doing at Public Health England is to try and find ways to measure the levels of loneliness, at a local level. This has been a challenge so far, but its absolutely critical if we want to track the effectiveness of the interventions to combat loneliness we implement.

In terms of interventions for loneliness, there's no 'one-size-fits-all' approach that will work all the time, for everyone. To tackle loneliness, we need a broad approach, matching the complexity of the issue with complex solutions.

Jude is particularly keen on ‘community-centred’ approaches, for many reasons. These are different from 'community-based' approaches, which is typically shorthand for 'out of the hospital'.

Approaches centred around the community are participatory. They empower people to have a greater say in their lives and health. They can involved local volunteers and peers to help provide support. They involve more partnerships and ways of 'co-producing' interventions, where healthcare providers and local communities work together.

Community-centred approaches also often build upon existing local ‘assets’. These are the things which communities already have which improve and maintain their health – local knowledge, individual people, community groups, the physical environment, and the local health care resources available.

Jude finishes her talk by setting out what needs to happen next. We need to get better at involving the local community in developing interventions, and implementing them. We also need to find ways to implement these local solutions at a much larger scale.

In terms of priorities for research, we need to test the effectiveness (and cost-effectiveness) of community-centred approaches – in particular in relation to loneliness and social isolation.

12:10 – Discussion

We now move onto the discussion of this symposium on loneliness.

First question, if we can 'fix' loneliness, is there evidence that it will directly impact on mental illness? Pam thinks that's it's possible, but we don't have enough research to back that up. What is more certain is that addressing (e.g.) a person's depression on its own, without addressing their loneliness., their loneliness will not go away.

Another question is about why sleep is affected by loneliness. Timothy suggests that sleep is a time where we are particularly alone, and so heightened to their thoughts and their anxiety of potential loneliness. Lonely people don't necessary sleep more or less than the general population, but the quality of their sleep is worse.

One more comment is around the types of loneliness that people feel throughout their life. The reasons why older people feel lonely are to do with social relationships being lost. The loneliness this causes may be more readily tackled with social interventions of the type that Jude was talking about. But the reasons why younger people feel lonely are more individual and personal, so perhaps the focus on developing interventions should be on young people.

Pam talks about work she did in the BBC Loneliness Experiment, the largest survey on loneliness ever conducted. She found that people had a range of solutions to combat loneliness, and she knows that young people have found this particularly useful, using it like a 'toolkit' to try and tackle their own loneliness.

A question from Twitter about loneliness in other age groups:

Jude thinks that this would be interesting to study. Many big events and transition points happen in our lives between adolescence and old age – parenthood, marriage and divorce, work and redundancy, all of which may have an impact on our social connectedness.

Time for a break for lunch. We’ll be back at 1.30pm, to carry on with another set of interesting talks.

13:30 – Symposium 2: “Tracking the life-long effects of mental illness”

With everyone fed and watered, we move onto our second symposium of the meeting, called ‘Tracking the life-long effects of mental illness’. Just like physical health, our mental health changes over time – understanding how this happens is crucial to preventing and treating mental illness.

Our chairs for this session are Prof Andrew McIntosh from University of Edinburgh, and Arden Tomison, who has developed a digital platform to support psychiatrists deliver the best diagnosis and care they can.

Arden explains that he has had his own "run-ins with mental ill-health". His view is that research – and data in particular – is going to be key to transforming mental health.

13:35 – Bill Fulford, “Montgomery and mental health: shared decision-making based on evidence and values”

First up to the speaker's podium in this session is Prof Bill Fulford from the University of Oxford. His work focuses on making sure that the values of people living with mental illness can be incorporated into decisions about the treatment they receive – a framework known as ‘Values-Based Practice’. You can read more about his work in a Q&A.

Today, Bill is talking about an important UK legal case Montgomery vs Lanarkshire Health Board – known as ‘Montgomery’. The facts of the case are fairly straightforward, but the implications of the UK Supreme Court’s judgement on the case in 2015 have had a profound effect on medicine.

A pregnant woman named Nadine Montgomery, who was under the care of her doctor Dr McLellan, developed diabetes during her pregnancy. Sadly, her baby suffered severe birth trauma because of shoulder dystocia, a well-known but rare complication of vaginal birth in mothers with diabetes.

Dr McLellan had decided not to raise the possibility of a caesarean section, because she considered the risks of that to be greater than the possibility of shoulder dystocia. Mrs Montgomery sued for damages citing medical negligence, and two lower courts had dismissed the case. However, the UK Supreme Court overruled these judgements, allowing Mrs Montgomery to appeal her case.

Bill considers that people have gotten the wrong end of the stick with the Montgomery judgement. Some doctors have considered that they now need to give all patients all the information available, which is not what the judgement was about.

As far as Bill is concerned, the upshot of Montgomery is that, when making decisions about treatment, doctors should not just consider what they think are the most important risks and benefits of treatment. They also need to consider what the individual patient would reasonably consider to be important – in short, the doctor needs to consider a person’s individual values. It is about shared-decision making.

Bill says that all that the Montgomery case has done is, in fact, made shared-decision making the legal basis of consent to treatments, in all areas of health care.

Bill goes onto to explain how those decisions are made. It depends, in his view, on a combination of evidence-based practice and something called values-based practice. Values-based practice is how doctors can make sure they are taking into account person's values: their needs, preferences, and desires, or in other words, the things that matter to them.

Bill gives an example of how values-based practice works, based on a true story. Mrs Jones was due to have surgery on to replace her knee for arthiritis. She was particularly keen gardener, but the prosthetic knee was going to affect her ability to kneel down for her gardening. Together, she and her surgeon decided to not go through with surgery, and instead had other treatment. Mrs Jones' mobility was more important to her, which meant she was prepared to avoid surgery if it meant she could carry on with a hobby she enjoyed.

Bill then gives another example where values-based practice is more challenging, this time in treatment for psychosis. The drugs are effective, but one common side-effect of these anti-psychotic drugs is weight gain. Should a doctor discuss with a person experiencing psychosis – who definitely needs treatment – the side-effect of weight gain, even if it means that person ends up not going through with the treatment? Montgomery would say yes – that decision should be a shared decision, not just about what the doctor thinks is important.

14:00 – Rogier Kievit, “It’s about time: Understanding how mental health changes"

The next speaker is Dr Rogier Kievit from the University of Cambridge. His talk is focussing on how it is important to understand how our brains and mental health changes over time, rather than focussing on single ‘snapshots’.

The traditional view of an illness like depression is that all the symptoms occur at the same time – all of which arise from the underlying cause of depression.

However, Rogier subscribes to a more dynamic view of mental illness. Symptoms, instead of all arising at the same time, each of the symptoms affects each other. For example, sleep problems lead to fatigue, which causes low mood, which leads to irritability – and it is this pattern of symptoms which is the depression. Tracking people's symptoms over time can give a much clearer picture of mental illness. If we just rely on a 'snapshot', we get a skewed picture of how mental ilness develops.

Rogier share's some research he's been involved in, which links people's cognitive abilities – things like memory – with their mental health.

He looked at a group of people, and how their memory and mental health changes over time. The first thing to notice – and it's pretty obvious – is that no two people are the same, or in Rogier's words "people vary in how they change".

Rogier and colleagues also looked at whether there was a link between a person's memory, and how their mental health changed. They found that for people with good memory, their mental health didn't decline as much.

By looking at people's cognitive abilities and their mental health over time – rather than just snapshots – they are trying to pin-point what causes what.

Rogier argues that taking a long-term view – more like a ‘video’ than a ‘snapshot’ – can improve our understanding of mental health, leading us to better ways to prevent and treat mental illness. It can also help us to understand key questions like why problems with mental health often occur together.

14:25 – Jessica Agnew-Blais, “Understanding the course of ADHD across the lifespan: the importance of longitudinal cohorts”

Next in this symposium is Dr Jessica Agnew-Blais from King’s College London. Her talk today is on Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and how studies have revealed it is not just a condition which affects children.

The symptoms of ADHD include inattention – for example, being easily distracted, or being forgetful in daily activities – and hyperactivity – for example, talking excessively. What's more, these symptoms are pervasive across all areas of life (e.g. at home and at school), and impair their day-to-day lives.

ADHD was originally thought of as a childhood disorder, which started in childhood and which kids eventually 'grow out of'. However, from the 1960s, researchers discovered that ADHD can persist into adulthood. Surveys have demonstrated that 2.5-5% of adults, or around 1 or 2 in every 40, have ADHD. The Guardian published an article in 2017 in which a few adults with ADHD tell their story of how they were diagnosed, and the effect it has on them.

And it's now being questioned whether it always starts childhood, or whether it can start in adulthood too.

Jessica shares some results from ‘longitudinal studies’, which track people over long periods of time. A recent study which Jessica led showed that about 22% of children who had ADHD went on to continue to have ADHD in adulthood – measured at age 18. They also identified a group of adults with ADHD who showed no symptoms of ADHD in childhood.

Jessica went on to study the long-term consequences of ADHD. She was interested in whether these depended on whether they developed ADHD in childhood or young adulthood, or whether it persisted throughout childhood and adulthood. She found that people had a higher risk of mental health problems if they had ADHD in adulthood, whether they had symptoms of ADHD in childhood or not.

So why do some people develop ADHD in childhood, and others in adulthood? And why does ADHD persist throughout their life in some people, but declines in others? A person's genes may hold a clue.

Jessica shares some innovative research using a study of twins, which can tease apart the influence of genes and environment on ADHD. Other researchers have identified tiny variations in our DNA which can affect our risk of ADHD. Jessica shows that the number of these variations that a person carries may influence how long their ADHD persists for.

The key to all of this work has been the longitudinal studies. These have helped to identify how ADHD and mental illness arise and develop over time.

14:50 – Philip Shaw, Understanding the different adult outcomes of childhood ADHD”

Our final talk in this symposium on tracking mental health over time is from Dr Philip Shaw from the National Human Genome Research Institute, USA. His talk is also focussing on ADHD and how it changes as children become adults.

It had previously been assumed that children with ADHD would eventually ‘grow out’ of the disorder by adulthood. As our previous speaker Jessica discussed, we now know that is not the always the case.

Philip describes two children he knew with ADHD. Both had the classic symptoms of ADHD, and were subsequently diagnosed with the disorder at age six. But the long-term after this point was very different. One had gone into 'remission', but the other, her ADHD had persisted well into childhood, and had severely impacted her adult life.

If we could understand more about how this happens – why ADHD goes into remission for some children, but doesn't for others – we may be able to develop new treatments which could ‘accelerate’ remission of ADHD in all children and adults.

What’s holding us back at the moment is that we’re not very good at predicting which children will ‘grow out’ of their ADHD and which will not, based upon the information we have right now. So instead, Philip studies genetics and brain imaging scans, to see if these can provide any insight into the long-term outlook for children with ADHD.

Philip has been using brain scan to study the brains of adults, some of whom had ADHD as a child, and some of whom still did as adults. He found that the brain 'reset' itself when ADHD goes into remission, such that it looks like those of people who have never had ADHD.

In another study of adults, he found that again, the 'wiring' of the brain in adults whose ADHD went into remission looked similar to that of adults who had never been affected. The brains of the adults who had persistent ADHD appeared to have many more connections between different parts of their brain.

However, in a final study, Philip shows that under certain circumstances, there are some differences in adults who had a history of ADHD, compared to those who had never had ADHD.

There's not a final answer yet as to how the brain changes over time as ADHD goes into remission or not. But finding this out could help to predict the long-term outlook for children with ADHD, and possibly could guide treatment for children and adults.

15:10 – Discussion

Now comes the time for a discussion on all the talks in symposium to do with tracking mental illness throughout life.

First question comes from Arden, our co-chair for this symposium. He asks a general question about how we could use data, particularly from private companies, to help advance research. Rogier suggests that apps could help with collecting data, as people tend to find they take a lot less effort than (for example) filling out a form. Jessica raises the point that apps may also help democratise science, reaching people who may otherwise be excluded. Philip also points out that interventions designed for children may also benefit from being apps – kids these days love their smartphones and tablets, and so an app-based treatment may actually be more acceptable to them.

Arden also asks Bill about the relationship between a person facing mental illness and a doctor – and how we can bring other people into the conversation about their values, for example a spouse or family member, or other healthcare professionals. Bill replies that conversations about the things that matter to us often arise 'out of nowhere', or when we're doing other things. There's challenges to taking the content of these conversations and bringing them into someone's care, for example confidentiality. But they can be overcome.

Another question is about ADHD, and its relationship with autism and anxiety – do any of the longitudinal studies reveal which comes first? The honest answer from Jessica and Philip is that we don't know – it is an area which would deserve more research.

We’ll take a break for tea and coffee – back in a jiffy.

16:00 – Rapid-fire talks

Our next session is new for 2019’s Mental Health Science Meeting – the Rapid-Fire Talks! Four speakers have five minutes to present their research to the audience. Start the clock, please…

16:05 – Shana Silverstein, “Observational fear learning: from mouse to man”

Our first rapid-fire talk is from Shana Silverstein, from National Institute of Mental Health & University College London. Her research focusses on ‘Observational Fear Learning’, how we develop a fear of something not by experiencing it ourselves but by observing others. Developing fears in this manner has implications for post-traumatic stress disorder and anxiety.

Shana has developed a method to study observational fear learning in mice. She has found that an area of the brain, called the 'dorso-medial prefrontal cortex' is crucial for the develop of observational fear learning.

Shana is now working on human studies. This work could shed light on the development of anxiety.

16:10 – Hjördis Lorenz, “Let the CAT out of the bag: A new measure to assess concrete and abstract thinking (CAT)”

Our next speaker is Hjördis Lorenz, from the University of Oxford.

Her research focus is on post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Hjördis explains that student paramedics are more likely than the general population to develop PTSD and depression.

One factor involved in this is rumination – the tendency to focus on a stressful event or situation, rather than on the solutions. Linked to rumination is the idea of two types of thinking – ‘concrete’ thinking, which focuses on the facts, and ‘abstract’ thinking, which involves a mental process to come to some sort of theory about those facts.

It has already been shown that less abstract thinking and more concrete thinking can reduce rumination – and in turn reduce depressive symptoms. However, currently there’s no way to measure how much concrete or abstract thinking someone is doing.

Hjördis and colleagues developed a new method to measure concrete and abstract thinking (CAT), which she tested in a group of student paramedics.

In their study, Hjördis showed that the method was reliable, and showed a correlation (as expected) between higher levels of abstract thinking with rumination and worry, as well as PTSD, depression and anxiety.

Hjördis is now testing the CAT measure in a larger group of emergency workers in a study funded by MQ.

16:20 – Stephanie Lewis, “Current Prevalence, Trends, and Gaps in Diagnoses in Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services: A Clinical Record Study”

The next rapid-fire talk is from Dr Stephanie Lewis from King’s College London.

Stephanie wanted to understand the mental health services provided for children and teenagers, as well as how these services – and the needs of these young people – has changed over time.

She did this studying the data from mental health records in 17-year olds from four boroughs in South London. Stephanie and her colleagues were able to estimate how many of these 17-year‑olds were diagnosed with a mental illness between 2008-2017. Stephanie found that in 2017, 6.5% of the 17-year olds in the South London area she studied were diagnosed with a mental illness – up from 3.5% in 2008.

She was also able to estimate the actual prevalence of mental illness in this population, using a nationwide study of over 2,000 British youths. Stephanie estimated that as many as 90% of the cases of mental illness in young people went undiagnosed.

Stephanie says that her research shows there is a ‘diagnosis gap’. More work is needed to understand the barriers that prevent young people from making use of mental health services in their area.

16:30 – Joshua Buckman, “The Stratified Medicine Approaches for Treatment Selection (SMART) Mental Health Prediction Tournament”

Our fourth and final Rapid-Fire Talk is from Dr Joshua Buckman from University College London.

Joshua’s talk is about a vision of ‘stratified medicine’ – where people facing mental illness receive a treatment that is best suited to them and is most likely to succeed. He’s focussing on Improve Access to Psychological Therapies (IAPT) programme, where people receive a high or a low intensity treatment.

There is a lot of data from people available to researchers which may hold clues as to which IAPT regime is likely to be successful for them or not. However, the challenge is finding ways to make the best us of all this data, to create a ‘model’ which can predict the likely future for that person.

Joshua decided to take an innovative approach and throw the question out into a ‘tournement’ he called the Stratified Medicine Approaches for Treatment Selection – or SMART for short.

13 teams from across the world were provided with data all about 4,000 people in the UK with mental illness, as well as the outcomes of the treatment which they received from the IAPT programme. They used this data to develop a computer program which could be used as a tool to predict which therapies were most appropriate for different groups of people.

The SMART tournament itself tested all the teams’ tools on further sets of data, representing a total of 33,000 people. The winner of the tournament was whichever team came up with the computer program which predicted the outcomes of these people with the greatest accuracy.

The next step is to translate the discoveries made from this tournement into clinical practise, such that every person recieves the most appropriate treatment, tailored for them.

16:40 – Keynote 2: Michael Ungar, “Diagnosing resilience: A multisystemic model for positive development in stressed environments”

Our final talk of the day is a keynote speech from Dr Michael Ungar from Dalhousie University in Halifax, Canada. His talk focusses on resilience in young people, and how it depends as much on the environment and the people around them, as it does on the individual themselves.

Michael starts with a video that his team filmed, of a couple of minutes of the life of a young man in foster care in Canada, as he spends time with his extended family at a house. He seems very happy and confortable, despite the adversity he faced earlier in childhood. Michael then reveals that the house he is in is a gang house, with a lot of drugs and guns in other rooms. The young man has developed coping mechanisms to deal with his environment and his life history – this is resilience.

Resilience has many dimensions. The traditional definition is about all about the individual – an individual’s ability to overcome adversity and continue their normal development. But for Michael, it's about much more that this. It's also about the ability for a person to navigate towards resources which can help them – and the environment that provides these resources.

Michael gives an example, from a trip he made to Japan after a recent tsunami. One of the first buildings to go back up was a small temporary building, which acted like a school, where the children could go to do their homework. This resource – known as a 'cram school' – was what the kids wanted: it was part of their normal lives before, and it helped many of them to recover from the trauma they had experienced.

We need to think of resilience in the context of a person's environment, not just about the person and their qualities.

He makes the point by talking about the story of 'Cinderella', which seen as a story of resilience. Michael jokes that really it should be renamed 'The Fairy Godmother', who provides the support that Cinderella needs to avoid life of misery, and instead ends up with a fairy-tale ending. Joking aside, it is the reality that plenty of people have abusive childhood, and some of these stories have the 'fairy-tale' endings, because of the environment around them and support they can get.

Michael shares some work he and his colleagues to try and develop a measure of this view of resilience. He also talks about some research his team did in 2012 to try and understand how children developed resilience followed abusive childhoods. They found that their experience of mental health services has a profound effect on their resilience.

Michael talks about how the abuse that children had experienced might also influence how effective certain interventions might be – for example, enrolling in group activities to reduce depressive symptoms and delinquency.

He also shares research that shows that interventions which increase resilience in children from unstable homes engage in less risky behaviour. However, for kids from stable homes, the opposite is actually true – building resilience actually increases risky behaviour.

These studies underline the idea that there is no 'one-size-fits-all' approach for building resilience in children. We have to take into their environment and history too.

This context is particularly important if we want to understand how to build resilience in particular groups – such as children in care, or refugees from war zones. We need to better understand their environment, and the resources available to them, if we're going to give every child the opportunity to thrive in the future.

Part of this is to increase the diversity in the people who are doing the research, as someone in our audience points out:

You can hear more from Michael in this interview with The Mental Elf:

That concludes day one of this year’s Mental Health Science Meeting. The audience will be heading off to the poster presentations – we'll be reporting on the highlights from this session later.

Come back tomorrow for another day of cutting-edge science and important discussions on the future of mental health research. Until then, have a good evening!

DAY TWO

Hello, and welcome back to the second day of the 2019 MQ Mental Health Science Meeting. Remember that you can follow updates here throughout the day, or by keeping an eye on the hashtag #MQScienceMeeting on Twitter.

09:00 – Symposium 3: “Mind and matter: intersections of physical and mental health”

The first session of day two is Symposium number three. The four speakers will be discussing the links between mental and physical health – how one can influence the other, and how we can break the link to improve wellbeing for everyone.

As we did in yesterday’s symposiums, we have two people chairing this session each with a unique perspective. We have Prof Matthew Hotopf, a researcher and psychiatrist from King’s College London, and Andrea Corbett, a wellness coach and mentor for young people.

Andrea opens the session by telling her story. She runs an organisation calleed FOCUS, but before this, she was 'Miss Corbett' – a school teacher. She was diagnosed with a mental illness, and was advised to go to the exercise – one thing lead to another and she ended up entering a bodybuilding competition – and won. She now mentors young people about the importance of looking after our physical health to help maintain our mental health.

You can read more about Andrea’s story and how bodybuilding helped her to turn her life around after being diagnosed with depression.

09:05 – Andrew Steptoe, “Mental health and the heart: how depression and anxiety influence, and are influenced by, heart disease”

Prof Andrew Steptoe from University College London is our first speaker in this symposium, talking about the link between heart disease and mental health.

Coronary heart disease is one of the most common health problems in the UK, and along with other heart conditions is responsible for a quarter of all deaths in the UK.

Andrew says that there is good evidence suggesting a link between heart disease and mental health issues, such as depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). What’s particularly interesting is that this relationship appears to go in both directions.

Mental ill-health, such as depressive symptoms, is associated with an increased risk of heart disease – about 30% increased risk. This is even the case when you take into account other risk factors, such as age, body weight, and exercise. And in turn, high levels of distress are common in people who have heart disease – which may lead to depression and anxiety.

Andrew sets out what might be the cause of this link. Something like a heart attack is a major life event, which may represent a brush with death which might cause people to reassess their life. Heart disease also causes changes to the body which may in themselves be causing mental illness. People also may develop PTSD as a result of the heart attack.

We also know that people with heart problems who are depressed are at increased risk of developing further heart problems in the future. In the short term, people who distressed immediately after a heart attack are more likely to be readmitted to hospital within 30 days. And in the long term, people with depressive symptoms after a heart attack are more likely to die than those without.

One explanation Andrew shares for this link is that people’s behaviours are affected by mental illness – for example, reducing physical activity, unhealthy diet, and not adhering to medical advice. People who have depressive symptoms are less likely to take medication like aspirin for heart disease. Andrew also suggests there is possibly a biological link – heart disease causes inflammation in the body, which is a known risk factor for depression.

So how do we manage depression in people with heart disease? Most likely, people will need the standard treatments depression – drugs and CBT. However, we have to be aware of the side-effects of some of these treatments, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) which can cause bleeding.

Andrew raises the possibility that depression in people with heart disease is different to depression in the general population, but that requires more research to understand.

09:30 – Bridget Callaghan, “Brain, Mind, Body: How our early environments shape brain-gut communication across development”

Second to speak in this symposium is Dr Bridget Callaghan, from Columbia University in New York. Bridget is sharing some fascinating research linking our brains, our emotions and behaviours, with the gut microbiome – the collection of bacteria and other microbes which live in our bowel.

Bridget begins with setting out that many of the disorders we consider to be developing in adulthood – both physical and mental health – in fact have their origins in early childhood. Part of this picture is our gut and how the bacteria respond to other events happening in our body.

The gut microbiome – the collection of bacteria living in our guts – respond to stressful events, which suggests that the gut and the brain are tightly connected. Bridget says that this also applies to disease as well as health. For example, depression and anxiety increase the risk of developing irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) later on in life – and vice versa.

Bridget has worked with children who are in orphanages and carehomes, who – even under the best of circumstances – face severe adversity. Researchers have shown that children who experience adversity in early life have increased risk of gastro-intestinal problems. In addition, those who have high levels of gastro-intenstinal problems have an increased risk of develop anxiety in early childhood.

Bridget's team looked closer at the microbiome of children, by analysing the different types of bacteria present in their poo. Children who had experience early life adversity had a less 'diverse' microbiome – fewer different types of bacteria. In addition, children who had experience stable homes had larger amounts of a bacteria family called lachnospiriaceae. In turn, this bacteria affected the way that children responded to threats, in an experiment involving showing them pictures of 'threatening' faces whilst in a MRI scanner.

Bridget and her team have also studied the link between the gut microbiome and mental health in rats. Her research suggests that it may be possibleto reduce the effect of early life adversity in rats by manipulating the gut microbiome using probiotic drinks – but this has not yet been proven in humans.

Bridget thinks that there is a huge amount of promise in manipulating the microbiome. This applies not just to gut disease, not even just for mental health, but for other conditions too, such as Parkinson's disease and multiple sclerosis.

However, there is a lot we don't know, and it'll require a huge amount of research to really unpick the link between the gut microbiome and our mental health.

09:55 – Clare Llewellyn, “Obesity: a matter of the mind, body and behaviour”

Next up in this symposium is Dr Clare Llewellyn from University College London, who is talking about the link between obesity and mental health.

About 1 in 4 adults in the UK are affected by obesity, and about 1 in 10 children starting in primary school are obese – becoming 1 in 5 by the time they leave primary school. Once a person develops obesity, it is then very difficult to return to a healthy weight – so Clare considers obesity as a chronic disorder.

Mental health problems often go hand in hand with obesity, particularly depression, partly because obesity itself can cause mental health problems, and vice versa. There are a wealth of different factors which link obesity and mental illness. These include social factors like poverty, biological factors like sleep and medication, psychological factors like self-esteem, and behavioural factors, like diet and physical activity.

Clare thinks that one of the most important factors is obesity stigma – negative attitudes towards others because of their weight – which is very common in the UK. People with obesity are commonly stereotyped as being weak-willed, lazy, greedy, and stupid. As a result, nearly 9 out of 10 people with obesity report they have been stigmatised, criticised or abused because of their weight.

What’s more, people with obesity report that some of this stigma has come from healthcare professionals. Clare says that this can result in people with obesity becoming disengaged with the healthcare system, and avoid lifestyle or psychological support, which in turn can make their physical and mental health worse.

At the core of obesity stigma is the belief that obesity is a ‘choice’ that an individual makes, and is entirely responsible for. There is a pervasive idea that it can be easily ‘fixed’ if the person ate less and exercised more. However, Clare says that these beliefs are over-simplistic.

Obesity is caused by many factors, including our genetics, our environment and culture, and complex social factors too, such as wealth and education. For some people, these factors ‘conspire’ together to make healthy choices and behaviours difficult to achieve. Clare takes us through a few of the most important factors which influence people's behaviours.

Our 'food environment' is a particular important factor. Clare shares research from America which shows that obesity in all groups of people increased from the late 1970s, right after deregulation on the amount of sugar that foods could contain.

Wealth is important too – children who come from poor backgrounds have higher rates of obesity than those from wealthier backgrounds. 'Healthy' food choices are relatively more expensive for poorer families than more affluent families. And fast food shops are more common in poorer areas than wealthier areas.

Clare says that our genetics may also play a part in obesity, though the relationship is quite complicated. Clare thinks that certain genes may influence our appetite – both how attracted we are to food, and how much food we can eat before we feel full. As one point of evidence for this – our appetites are pretty consistent throughout life, include after birth, where some breast-fed babies are 'hungrier' than others.

Clare concludes by saying that to tackle obesity, and the mental health problems it causes, we must tackle obesity stigma. We need to educate everyone – including healthcare professionals – about the real causes of obesity. We also need to raise awareness of the link between mental health and obesity, such that researchers in both areas should be talking to each other more to understand how their work overlaps.

One comment from the audience following Clare's talk says that some of the medicines used to treat certain mental illnesses can themselves cause weight gain – which adds another layer to this complex relationship.

10:25 – Simon Gilbody, “Smoke free mental health services: from rhetoric to reality”

Our final speaker in this symposium on the cross-over between physical and mental health is Prof Simon Gilbody from the University of York. Simon is focussing on how we can help people facing mental illness to quit smoking.

After a brief lesson in post-modern architecture, Simon begins by setting out the importance of helping people to stop smoking – in terms of things we can do to reduce our risk of early death or poor health, quitting smoking should the number one priority.

However, the smoking rate amongst people facing severe mental illness is two to three times higher than the general population. One of the things which had previously contributed to this was a 'culture of smoking' within mental health services, where people began smoking as they used these services.

Simon says smoking contributes to long-term health conditions faced by many people with poor mental health, who have a reduced life expectancy by 20 to 25 years, on average. It also contributes to poverty, as cigarette smoking is an expensive habit. What's more, the stigma that has developed around smoking also contributes to mental illness (in the same way that obesity stigma does, as we heard from Clare's talk earlier).

Simon turns to some "mythbusting" around why smoking is more common amongst people with severe mental illness. Firstly – the myth that people smoke to reduce stress – not true. Secondly – the myth that smoking can counteract the side-effects of medication – but people who quit smoking end up taking less medication.

A third myth is that there is never a good time to quit, because quitting smoking will lead to relapse of mental health problems. The final myth is that people don't want to quit – that is also not true.

How can we help people with mental illness to quit smoking? Simon says that the same things which work for the general population, also work for smokers – behavioural and nicotine replacement therapies. But the main problem for people facing mental illness is access to these services.

Simon moves on a trial he ran called SCIMITAR, which aims to develop a smoking cessation programme, tailor-made for people with severe mental illness. In the small pilot study for SCIMITAR, they managed to double the 'quit-rate' for the people in the study.

The results from the SCIMITAR+ trial – the largest trial ever conducted into stop smoking services for people with severe mental illness – will be released very soon. But Simon reveals that the results from the pilot study appear to hold up when they look at larger numbers of people with severe mental illness.

Simon finishes by promoting a programme called ‘Closing the Gap’, a research network looking to reduce the inequality in physical health for people facing severe mental illness.

10:50 – Discussion

It's now time for discussion on all the talks on the link between mental health and physical health.

First question is about stigma – but specifically about the discrimination that people with mental illness face. Does that also contribute to physical health? Andrea says that she faced discrimination as a black woman, she was immediately prescribed medication without any discussion about the possibility of cognitive behavioural therapies. Andrew says that doctors whose specialty is in physical health (such as heart health) aren't as familiar with mental health problems, and how they may be treated and prevented. Simon and Clare confirm that for access to services to support people to quit smoking or lose weight, sadly there is some subtle discrimination against people with mental illness.

Another question was about the efforts that people make to improve their physical health – many people carry with them a 'fear of failure', that their efforts may not work or they won't sustain them. Does that contribute to mental illness? Andrew says that the message we should be getting across to people, in the example of physical activity, is that any level of physical activity is better than none. Andrea says that with the young people she works with, it's not about just sending them to the gym – some of them play golf or do other activities to achieve their goals.

We have a question about the genetic effects of these links. Clare says that there are about 1,000 different genetic variations which are linked to obesity, and scientists are trying to work out the biological effects of these variations. The problem is that the vast majority of these variations only make a tiny difference on their own. But Clare says it's not really the direction we should be heading – focussing on the environment will benefit a lot more people that just focussing on the the genetics behind obesity.

You can hear more from Bridget, Clare, and Simon in this interview with The Mental Elf:

Time for a quick break for tea and coffee. Back at 11.30 for more important talks and discussions.

11:30 – Keynote 3: Joshua Gordon, “NIMH priorities and progress"

Our next talk, and our third and final Keynote speech, is from Dr Joshua Gordon. As Director of the USA’s National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), Joshua has a unique overview of the opportunities – and the challenges – for mental health research, which he is sharing with us today.

Joshua sets out the complexity of problems faced by mental health research. To illustrate, he shows the audience a picture of a 'rosehip neuron', a type of cell in the brain. It has dozens of little branches, and at the end of these branches it connects to hundreds of other nerve cells, and there are thousands of proteins involved in these interactions. The billions of cells in our brain, with trillions of connections between these cells, is what causes our behaviour, and mental illness. Joshua says that NIMH set itself a mammoth task to understand all of this complexity.

Joshua moves onto one priority area for NIMH's work – neural circuits, which are basically how our brains are wired. Neural circuits are groups of cells in our brain which are connected together to carry out a particular task. These circuits can sometimes go wrong however, which can contribute to several conditions, including Parkinson’s disease and some mental health conditions.

Joshua says that studying these neural circuits has traditionally been difficult, because the tools available to do so are fairly blunt – they may switch on or off large areas of the brain at once. But with new techniques and imaging, we're much more able to understand how these neural circuits affect our behaviour.

As an example, Joshua shares some research in mice. Using a virus to force nerve cells in mice to produce particular molecules, scientists can test how particular areas of the brain are involved in anxiety. This research shows that blocking the specific connection between two brain regions makes the mice much less anxious – they are more likely to explore open spaces, which is not a normal behaviour for mice.

The challenge of this research is now to translate these findings into humans, to learn more about human behaviour and mental illness. Even if we are able to show something happens in mice, it doesn't necessarily mean the same happens in human brains. For technical and ethical reasons, scientists are not able to do the same experiments in humans as we might do in mice, so new approaches are needed.

Another priority for NIMH is an approach called 'computational psychiatry'. Researchers are looking at mental health at multiple different levels. Some are looking at the small scale – the genes, molecules, and cells involves – whilst others are looking at the ‘high-level’ experiences – the behaviours and the social impacts of mental illness. This is a real strength for mental health research, but it also presents challenges – for any one condition, or for any one person, how do we bring together the research at all these levels, to make sense of what’s going on, and how to treat them?

Computer science can help to address these challenges. Scientists can use computers to bring together data at all these different ‘levels’ of a disease, so that they can figure out how the ‘high level’ experiences arise from the interactions at the smaller end of the scale.

Joshua gives an example of research into reward-predicting and mood. It involves a smartphone app, with a basic gambling game, where the action was interspersed with asking about the player's mood. The researchers were able to show that happiness was linked to reward-prediction behaviour. But what's more, they were able to link the areas of the brain involved in mood and reward prediction. This can help to understand how the link between depression and reward-seeking behaviour, something that is known to be affected in depression.

Joshua also shares some other research which shows that MRI scans of people's brains may be able to identify whether people with depression will respond to a particular treatment or not.

Joshua moves onto the third priority area for NIMH, suicide prevention. Suicide is one the biggest causes of death in people in the US – particularly in young people. He shares research which shows that screening for suicidal thoughts in hospital emergency rooms – by asking about people's suicidal thoughts – can significantly reduce the number of subsequent suicide attempts. He also shares some provocative research which suggests that the drug ketamine can dramatically and quickly reduce suicidal thoughts. Finally, Joshua talks about a study hoping to predict the risk that people will attempt suicide.

Joshua's final message is that research can provide hope for the future, but applying this science to help people facing mental illness as soon as possible, that is the real challenge.

You can hear more from Joshua in this interview with The Mental Elf.

12:20 – Panel Discussion: “Can research ‘move the needle’ on mental illness?”

The next session is a panel discussion on how research can ‘move the needle’ on mental illness.

The attention paid to mental health across the world has never been higher. Members of the Royal family, celebrities, business leaders and Governments are speaking out and committing to action. But the scale of the challenge is huge. And a lack of progress in our understanding and treatment of mental illness means we haven’t yet seen the advances necessary.

Research has the potential to transform the status quo – as it has done for cancer or HIV/AIDS. But mental health is a long way off seeing similar steps forward. Levels of funding for and awareness of research into mental health are huge barriers.

But does the issue run deeper than this? After leaving his role as Director at the National Institute of Mental Health, USA, Tom Insel said in an interview:

"I spent 13 years at NIMH really pushing on the neuroscience and genetics of mental disorders, and when I look back on that I realise that, while I think I succeeded at getting lots of really cool papers published by cool scientists at fairly large costs – I think $20 billion – I don’t think we moved the needle in reducing suicide, reducing hospitalizations, improving recovery for the tens of millions of people who have mental illness."

So how do we ‘move the needle’? How do we translate the outcomes of research into tangible benefits for people facing mental illness? Do we need to change our research priorities? Or maybe the way that research is done?

All of the questions above will be addressed by a panel of speakers. The session will begin with a two-minute statement from each of the panel members. Then, the discussion will open up to questions from members of the audience, and the chair of the session, Kate Kelland, Health & Science Correspondent for the news agency Reuters.

We begin with an opening statement from Prof Louise Arseneault, from King’s College London. As far as Louise is concerned, the key is to try and get the discoveries made into the public, to make changes to healthcare, but we don't know enough about how best to do that. The model of 'co-production' – where scientists and doctors work with people affected by mental illness to develop new interventions – will also be important, but again, it is difficult to know exactly how to do that.

Next to speak is Dr Sarah Carr from University of Birmingham. Sarah thinks that mental health research should cut across all different areas of researchers. We should do better to break out of our bubbles, and work with others from outside of the mental health research, including people from the arts and humanities. We also need to diversify how we fund research, such as working with the voluntary sector. Finally, we should involve people affected by mental illness more. That doesn't just mean have someone on the research funding panel – we could involved people facing mental illness in deciding the priorities for research, and even in doing the research itself.

Following Sarah is Dr Joshua Gordon from the USA’s National Institute of Mental Health, who gave the keynote speech in the previous session. In response to his predecessor's comment that we haven't made a difference in mental health care – Joshua think's he's "dead wrong". Research is making a difference, but the problem as Joshua sees it is that it's not making a difference fast enough. We need to set goals – short, medium, and long-term – which will speed up how translates research into benefit for people with mental illness.

Next is Prof Tim Kendall, the National Clinical Director for Mental Health for NHS England. Tim thinks we're unlikely to see the major breakthroughs in the future – the 'silver bullets' that can treat huge numbers of people. Funding for mental health services is absolutely key, particularly in North and West of England, where they are lacking. Tim thinks we also need to change our focus, to do more research into prevention and understanding mental illness. And again, we need to do better at translating our research into benefit for people facing mental illness – there are pockets of great work doing just this, but they need to be rolled out further.

Our final panellist is Andrew Welchman from the Wellcome Trust, a charity which has made funding of mental health research a priority. Andrew states that he is optimistic – organisations like Wellcome and MQ are investing more and more into mental health research. But we need to continue to "make the case" for funding mental health research to get everyone "singing from the same hymn sheet". Building this broad research community is a major part of MQ's work and one of the purposes of the Mental Health Science Meeting.

Now Kate invites questions from the audience.

The first question asks why people with mental illness are not invited to take part in clinical trials, in the same way that people with cancer are? Andrew says that the uptake of clinical trials for psychiatry is way too low, and the people entering these trials are not representative of the whole population. Tim makes the point that the funding disparity between cancer and mental health research is stark – £1,000 is spent on cancer research per cancer patient per year, but for mental health research, that figure is more like 10p. Louise makes the point that in the same way that cancer research is focussing on helping people live with cancer, mental health research should also focus on helping people live with their condition.

Joshua and Kate raise the question of whether the language we use might affect how we see mental illness – saying that people have mental health 'issues' or 'problems', as opposed to 'illness', might belittle mental health and make it seem like it isn't a priority for research funding.

Tim says that the conversation around mental health is changing in the UK. As an example, he says that nowhere else in the world would you see on tabloid newspapers being sympathetic to mental health conditions. Andrew agrees that we have seen a massive shift in attitudes towards mental health – in the media and in government priorities.

Kate asks the panel if there is any particular priority areas where we should be investing our money.

Louise says that the focus should be on prevention – treatment is important, but for the long-term, the mental health of children and young adults will be absolutely key. Joshua talks more about screening which he discussed in his talk, in relation to screening for suicidal thoughts. However, Joshua says that screening programmes can be challenging to develop depending on the type of mental illness we're trying to tackle. Tim thinks that schools are promising settings for mental health awareness, screening, and interventions. Sarah thinks that prevention is key, but we need to consider people struggling with their mental health throughout their life – for them, prevention of mental health crises should be the focus.

Another question from the audience is about setting ourselves goals. The UN has 'global development goals' for tackling poverty – could we set ourselves similar ambitious targets to help us all move in the right direction? Kate invites the panel to contribute ideas for some goals.

Joshua says one goal would be to get a test – any test – which can predict which treatment which be beneficial for a particular person with a mental illness. But setting goals, and then making research decisions to acheive those goals, are two very different things.

One more question from the audience is about the 'implementation gap' – the time it takes for research findings to actually be implemented in the health service and make a difference to people's lives. How can researchers make changes to their research to make it more likely to be implemented? Louise talks about research networks which are already trying to do this implementation work. Sarah thinks that scientists ought to be thinking at the beginning of a research study, rather than at the end of the study, how their work will be implemented.

A final question is about how people with lived experience of mental illness can contribute to research. How can we ensure this involvement is not 'tokenistic', and how can we remove the barriers which prevent people from getting involved? Sarah thinks that research should be 'co-produced' – design and carried out by people with mental illness in collaboration with scientists. People with mental illness have a massive amount to contribute, in their knowledge and their experience. We should reduce the bureaucracy that universities throw up to get more people involved.

That brings the panel discussion to a close, and I’m sure there will be plenty more discussion over lunch. We’ll be back at 2.15pm for the final sessions of the meeting.

14:15 – Rapid-Fire Talks

Certain to wake up anyone feeling a little sluggish after lunch – it’s the Rapid-Fire Talks! We’re following the same format as yesterday – four speakers have just five minutes to share their research highlights. On your marks…

14:15 – Alexandra Schmidt, “Physical activity and depression: Do the beneficial effects extend to individuals with adverse childhood experiences (ACEs)?”

Our first speaker in today’s Rapid‑Fire Talks is Alexandra Schmidt from the University of Sussex.

We already know that physical activity can help people deal with depression. What we don’t know enough about is whether it can also help people at risk of depression – specifically, people with adverse childhood experiences.

Alexandra explains that adverse childhood experiences include situations like domestic violence, experiencing neglect or abuse, a family member being in prison, or growing up in a household with alcohol or drug addiction.

Alexandra used data from the National Survey of Health and Development – a group of more than 5,000 people followed throughout their lives.The results of her study confirmed that more physical activity is associated with less depressive symptoms.

Interestingly, she found that people who had a particular type of adverse childhood experience benefited more from physical activity in adolescence, and reduced their risk of developing depression later in life. This suggests physical activity can protect against depression, and could form the basis of an intervention for young people who experienced adversity in childhood.

14:25 – Urška Košir, “Relationship between physical symptoms and psychological well-being in adults with soft tissue sarcoma”

Our next Rapid-Fire Talk is from Urška Košir from University of Oxford. Her research focuses on the mental wellbeing of people with soft tissue sarcomas, a rare type of cancer.

These tumours often require invasive medical procedures to preserve life and limb, but which often result in disfigurement or amputation. Pain and fatigue are very common in people with these sarcomas, and up to 50% of patients with these cancers report long-term disability. All of these experiences are factors which are known to increase a person’s risk of mental illness. Therefore, Urska wanted to explore the experiences of people living with soft tissue sarcomas, and the impact these tumours had on their mental wellbeing.

Conducting interviews with a group of 28 people with sarcomas, she found that many of the patients had experience significant disfigurement, which led them to feel self-conscious. But they were mostly concerned with the functionality of their limbs before their appearance. People who had concerns about their body image were more likely to have a lower mood.

Urska believes that her study provides evidence that health care professionals treating sarcomas should consider the impact that surgical procedures can have on a person’s mental wellbeing. She believes that interventions which help these patients develop and improve their ability to cope with possible disfigurement may have a positive impact on their mental health.

14:35 – Radhika Kandaswamy, “DNA methylome marks of exposure to psychosocial stress during adolescence: Analysis of a novel longitudinal MZ discordant twin study”

Our third talk in this Rapid-Fire session comes from Dr Radhika Kandaswarmy from King’s College London.

Radhika starts by sharing some fascinating evidence that stressful events in our lives can leave a ‘fingerprint’ on our DNA. This fingerprint – chemical changes to the DNA known as ‘epigenetic’ changes – is different in people who have been exposed to adverse experiences, compared to those who haven’t. Chemical changes may affect how active particular genes are, so this evidence points to a potential a biological mechanism for how stressful experiences have an impact on our mental health.

However, Radhika says that the evidence so far hasn’t been reliable enough to be useful – it has come studies which were too small, or from very specific groups, which make it difficult to generalise the findings for a wider group of people.

Radhika shares work that she has carried out using a group of 118 twin pairs who take part in a study called ‘E-Risk’. She carefully chose pair twins where one twin was exposed to adverse experiences and one was not. As this groups of twins share identify DNA, Radhika was able to look at the impact of adverse experiences in adolescence and be more confident that the effects were purely down to the experiences rather than any other factors. The twins in this study had been asked to give cheek swabs and blood samples throughout their childhood between the ages of 5 and 18.

Her results show that there is indeed a specific fingerprint left on the DNA by adverse experiences, which also change during childhood. This work contributes to our understanding of the consequences that stressful events can have on our genetic code, and ultimately our mental health, during adolescence.

14:40 – Sophie Li, “Are women’s catastrophic beliefs ovary-actions? Progesterone levels predict reductions in phobic avoidance following cognitive restructuring in women with spider phobia”

The final speaker in our Rapid‑Fire Talks session is Dr Sophie Li from the University of New South Wales in Australia.

Sophie is focussing today on the fear of spiders in women. Phobias like this result in ‘catastrophic beliefs’ – the irrational belief that some sort of worst-case scenario will certainly happen if that person is exposed to the thing they fear – in this case, spiders.

Exposure therapy – where people are exposed to the thing they fear in a controlled setting – can form part of treatment which can help to reduce anxiety, and ultimately their phobia. However, Sophie explains that the results for this in women are not consistent. There is evidence to suggest that the reason for this variability are hormone produced by the ovaries – namely, oestrogen and progesterone.

In her study, Sophie measured the levels of progesterone and estradiol (a type of oestrogen) in blood samples given by people undergoing exposure therapy for spider phobias. She then looked at the levels of these hormones in relation to the outcome of the therapy – whether the women avoided spiders less often, or reported that their fear of spiders had reduced.

She found that increased level of progesterone (but not estradiol) were associated with less avoiding of spiders in these women – they were able to move closer to a box with a lab spider called "Little Miss Muffet". However, neither hormone was associated with a reduction in fear of spiders that these women reported.

Levels of progesterone change during a woman’s menstrual cycle. Sophie says that her research suggests that exposure therapy for women with anxiety may be more successful if the therapy was timed to coincide with a peak in progesterone in the blood during her monthly cycle. It also raises the possibility that increasing the levels of progesterone using drugs may increase the effective of exposure therapy in women – though more research is needed to prove this.

14:50 – Symposium 4: “Tackling suicide and self-harm”

Our final session of the Mental Health Science Meeting is looking at suicide and self-harm – one of the most emotive topics in mental health, but also one of the most important. The researchers are discussing how we predict who is most likely to attempt suicide, and how we can prevent this from happening.

Chairing this session is Gregor Henderson, Director for Wellbeing and Mental Health for Public Health England, and Paul McGregor, a mental health campaigner, advocate and author.

Paul begins the session by sharing his experience. He says his story is really about his dad. Paul says that his dad had everything on paper – a good job, good physical health. But when Paul was 18, his dad completely changed, to someone very distant. Paul's dad was prescribed antidepressant drugs, and later attempted suicide. His dad's depression got worse, and he struggled as he received treatment in a mental health unit. Sadly, his dad took his own life – six months after he was first diagnosed with depression. What made Paul feel even worse after this event, was that he was never able to answer the question 'why' – why his dad took his own life. Accepting this helped Paul move on, but also made him a strong advocate for research into suicide.

You can read more about Paul’s experience and his hopes for the future.

15:00 – Rory O’Connor, “Psychological Processes and Suicidal Behaviour”

Prof Rory O’Connor from the University of Glasgow is our first speaker in this symposium. Rory focusses on understanding the psychology behind suicidal thoughts and actions, and hopes to identify ways to prevent suicide.

Rory came in to chat to us with Paul McGregor (the co-chair for this session), you can listen to their conversation on our podcast.

Researchers have been trying for a long time to define exactly how suicide develops. Rory today is going to try and get across one aspect of this – the thought processes that an individual person goes through when they are considering suicide.

We know that 90% of people who die by suicide have a recognisable mental illness. However, on the flip side, the vast majority of people with mental illnesses do not attempt suicide – that figure is maybe 4-5%. Rory say's particularly interested in that 4-5% of people with mental illness, who go on to attempt suicide.

One key component of many recent explanations is the link between suicidal thoughts and suicidal actions (i.e. suicide attempts). The theory behind this is that having suicidal thoughts, and these thoughts progressing into actions, are two separate stages, and have different explanations behind them.

In 2011, Rory published a scientific paper which presented his method of explaining the progression from suicidal thoughts to behaviours – known as the ‘integrated motivational-volitional model of suicidal behaviour’ or the IMV model for short. He has since updated this IMV model with the help of Olivia Kirtley from University of Leuven in Belgium.

Briefly, the IMV model has three phrases:

  1. A pre-motivational phase – where various factors, including a person’s upbringing and specific ‘trigger’ events, make someone more likely to develop suicidal thoughts.
  2. A motivational phase – where suicidal thoughts emerge, as a result of feeling defeated and trapped.
  3. A volition phase – where suicidal thoughts make the transition into behaviours and actions, including suicide attempts.

Rory presents some research where he asked people who had attempted suicide about a range of factors, and then followed them for another four years to see if they attempt suicide again. One of the most important factors which could be used to predict whether someone would attempt suicide again was feelings of 'being trapped'.

Rory then focusses on the transition from having thoughts about suicide, to acting and attempting suicide. Rory's IVM model puts forward the theory that there's eight 'volitional' factors which influence that transition, and his research has shown that in a group of Scottish young people, these factors do indeed predict who is most likely to make suicide attempts.

Rory moves onto talking about some interesting research into the biology of suicide, in particular the hormone cortisol, known as the 'stress hormone'. Reseach that Rory conducted with his twin brother (who is a stress researcher) shows that people who have attempted suicide in the past have a 'blunted' response to stress, with lower levels of cortisol when exposured to stressful events.

Rory finishes his talk by discussing an aspect of our thought processes which is understudied in relation to suicide – the 'automatic system', which is responsible for our unconscious, impulsive behaviour. In a study, Rory and his colleagues were able to show that people who unconsciously focus more on death than life are more likely to attempt suicide in the future.

Rory ends his talk by reaffirming that suicide research will need an approach that mixes science from lots of different areas – he says we all have a role to play in suicide prevention.

15:25 – Becky Mars, “Transitions from suicidal thoughts to attempts”

Following Rory in this symposium is Dr Becky Mars from University of Bristol. Becky is discussing how young people who have suicidal thoughts make the transition to making suicide attempts.

Becky reiterates that for everyone who has suicidal thoughts, only a small number of them will go on to act on these thoughts. Research has told us which people are more likely to have suicidal thoughts, but we don't have any good measures to predict who will go onto to make a suicide attempt. Knowing this would help doctors to target interventions at people most like to act on their suicidal thoughts, and hopefully prevent them from dying.

Becky goes on to discuss different 'models' of suicide – different theories which try to explain how people develop suicidal thoughts, and then how they make the transition to attempting suicide. These models (which include the IVM model our previous speaker Rory developed), have varying amounts of evidence to back them up.

For her research, Becky has been using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), also known as the ‘Children of the 90s’ study, which charts the health of 14,500 families in the Bristol area. Each participant in the study has provided a huge amount of data which researchers like Becky can use to identify risk factors for all kinds of health conditions, such as suicidal thoughts and attempts.

Becky has identified several factors which can influence the chances that someone who has had suicidal thoughts, or has self-harmed, will go on to make a suicide attempt. Those factors include substance misuse, having friends or family who have self-harmed, and certain personality traits.

Becky then talks about another aspect of her work with the 'Children of the 90s' study, which is only made possible because the participants are tracked over time. She tried to uncover whether, for people who had suicidal thoughts at age 16, if there's anything which predicts if they would then go on to attempt suicide later in life. The results from that work will be released soon.

15:45 – Ellen Townsend, “The Card Sort Task for Self-Harm: An innovative method for investigating the complexity and temporal dynamics of self-harm”

Next to speak in this symposium on self-harm and suicide is Prof Ellen Townsend from the University of Nottingham. She is talking about a technique she developed with her colleagues which could help to identify the factors which lead a person to self-harm.

Firstly, Ellen makes the case that we should be careful with our language around self-harm and suicide. Nobody 'commits' suicide, they 'die by suicide'. And we shouldn't refer to 'self-harmers', we should recognise them as people. One of Ellen's team, Emma Nielsen, has written an excellent blog on this topic of language.

Ellen focusses on young people who self-harm, partly because there is a massive increase in the rates of self-harm in adolescence, but also because it strongly linked to suicide – the leading cause of death in young people.

In the UK, 200,000 people self-harm every year – which is four times as many as people as who ran the London Marathon in 2017. But Ellen says this is just the 'tip of the iceberg' of people who self-harm, without making contact with mental health services.

The technique Ellen has developed is called the Card Sort Task for Self‑Harm, or CaTS for short. It allows people who have self-harmed to share the complexity of their experience, and also show the 'sequence of events'. For Ellen, the order of events really matters.

People who have self-harmed are presented with a set of 117 cards, each with a different statement on them. The cards were divided in five categories: Thoughts (e.g. ‘I could not trust anyone’), Feelings (e.g. ‘I felt trapped’), Events (‘I had an argument with my friend’), Behaviours (‘I was not able to sleep’), and Services & Support (‘I talked to a teacher which helped’).

The people are then asked to think of either their first or their most recent incidence of self-harm. They are asked assign cards in a time sequence, ranging from 6 months before they self-harmed, up to the event itself, and then afterwards.

Ellen says that the CaTS task allows people to tell their own story of their experience of self-harm. Importantly, it was also developed with people who have lived experience of self-harm. Ellen is very passionate about 'co-production', where people with lived experience of mental illness work together with researchers to develop new interventions.

For the researchers, it could help to identify common events and feelings leading up to self-harming, which could present key opportunities at which to intervene. One common sequence which Ellen found was that the idea of 'impulsiveness' occured frequently before self-harm – acting on impulse without planning.

When people were asked to reflect on their most recent experience of self-harm, there was a tendancy for people to report that they felt they wanted to die. In addition, people no longer reported that they felt 'relieved' after self-harm, which was a common feeling after the first ever attempt of self-harm. Ellen says that this suggests that how people feel about self-harm deteriorates over time as they repeatedly self-harm, which is concerning, as it could lead people to attempt suicide in the future.

Ellen has subsequently created an online version of CaTS, known as e-CaTS. This will mean that more young people will be able to share their experience of self-harm, and so researchers like Ellen will be able to better understand the process of self-harm.

The young people who have been involved in developing and testing the CaTS tool have also found it useful. Ellen says that many of them have reported a 'light-bulb moment', and now better understand their own thought processes.

16:15 – Reinhard Lindner, “Multimorbidity and the wish for assisted suicide”

Our final speaker for this symposium is Prof Reinhard Linder from the University of Kassel in Germany. His talk is focussing on ‘physician assisted suicide’ and is working to identify the factors which influence the decisions of those who request it.

‘Physician-assisted suicide’ – requesting help from a medical professional to end one’s own life – is often linked to a struggle with multiple diseases – and has an interesting link to this morning's symposium on the link between physical and mental health.

Reinhard explains that the desire for help to end one’s life isn’t really connected to the severity of a particular disease, or the combined severity of many diseases – but is actually about the psychological burden of these diseases, and losing the ability to cope. Studies have reported that about 5% of elderly people with multiple health complaints have requested assisted suicide.

Reinhard thinks it's important to distinguish between different thoughts around death. Many older people with multiple health problems have a 'tiredness of life' – a desire to die but without any action. Others report 'death wishes', which take on many forms. Some 'accept' death, in the sense of being satisfied with their life, whilst others wish the natural process of death to start earlier, but without wanting it to be accelerated.

However, in the case of assisted suicide, people often feel like they wish to speed up death, as a result of feeling like they are a burden to others. Research has shown that these people are likely to have had suicidal thoughts earlier in life as well.

Reinhard says that the question of assisted suicide and euthanasia is important and timely. We're now more able to treat people with severe physical illnesses, such that their live is extended, but sadly for many of these people the quality of their life may not change. In many countries and regions across the world, assisted suicide and euthanasia is legal. Here in the UK, there are ongoing, passionate debates about assisted suicide.

Reinhard sets out what we know about the factors which make people more likely to request physician-assisted suicide. One group of these factors are psychological. For example, Reinhard explains that a feeling of ‘losing control’, as well as the fear of death, and 'fear of the unknown', are factors which make people more likely to request assisted suicide. Other factors, such as holding strong religious beliefs, make people less likely to request assisted suicide.

There are also ‘social’ factors which affect the likelihood of requesting assisted suicide. Factors such as ageism, availability of access to support networks, quality of social networks, and economic burden, all have a role to play in someone’s wishes to request assisted suicide.

Reinhard says that to prevent suicide in groups of people like this, we need to focus on psychosocial interventions. Specific training for healthcare professionals and relatives of people with multiple health problems may also be useful to help address these issues.

16:40 – Discussion

We now move to a discussion around the previous talks on suicide and self-harm.

One question is about whether bereavement is a risk-factor for requests for assisted suicide – particularly in older people who lose spouses. Reinhard says that it certainly is possible, and that we should offer more support to people who lose loved ones later in life.

Ellen answers a question about where we should be focussing on research funding in relation to self-harm and suicide. She believes that it depends on the exact challenge we're trying to address, but she is a fan of community-based approaches.

Paul asks a question about the 'ripple effect' of suicide – that exposure to suicide in family or friends is a risk factor. How strong is that risk factor? Rory says that, like suicidal thoughts, not everyone who loses someone to suicide will themselves try to take their own life, but the effect is particularly strong up to five years after that person's suicide.

Paul finishes the discussion with a final question. How can we educate children to keep them safe? Ellen says we should take what young people say seriously. We do young people a diservice if we don't listen to them.

We now close the meeting with some awards. For poster presentations, they are: Ruth Roberts from University College London, and Radhika Kandaswamy, from King's College London. There is a travel prize for Sophie Li from of New South Wales in Australia. And an 'audience vote' prize for Faith Matcham from King's College London. Congratulations to them all!

And with that, the MQ Mental Health Science Meeting for 2019 draws to a close. It’s been a fantastic meeting with some great science and intense discussion.

Thanks to everyone who has contributed to the conversation on Twitter via the #MQScienceMeeting hashtag, and thanks to everyone who’s been reading the live blog.

Next week, we’ll be sharing a round-up of the most important learnings from the meeting. Until then, thank you, and see you next year!

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

Support for Carers

This was an interesting article I found on: Talk Health Partnership

See credits below.


Unpaid carers are a lifeline to those they look after and are of huge benefit to society as a whole. A staggering 7 million of us in the UK are carers and so the relief to NHS services is truly incalculable.

Supporting carers and enabling them to care effectively and lovingly is vital. Our home carers are our forgotten workforce and the mainstay of many people in society. It is incredibly important to ensure that our carers are themselves cared for; their wellbeing is fundamental to ensuring they can continue to sustain the lives of some of the most vulnerable in our communities.

Who counts as a carer?

You may well be a carer without realising it.

Being a carer does not necessarily mean you are caring for someone twenty-four hours a day. You may be doing less hours but still being of critical support to someone.

The following criteria can help you recognise your role. According to the NHS, if you perform any of the following, you are technically a carer:

  1. Regularly looking after someone because they are ill, elderly or disabled.
  2. Generally helping someone with washing, dressing or taking medicines.
  3. Helping with shopping, cleaning and laundry.
  4. Helping someone to pay bills and organise finances.
  5. Providing emotional support by sitting together to keep them company or watching over someone if they can’t be left alone.

Sadly, carers are often under-recognised and suffer many hardships, often including loneliness, themselves.

This is worrying for the health of the seven million of us who care. On top of this, there is also a dreadful knock on effect harming the vulnerable people they care for. Ill physical or mental wellbeing in a carer can – understandably – lead to neglect of the person requiring care. In extreme cases, elder or other types of abuse occur.

It is clear that carers require far more help than they currently receive. The facts are stark:

  • 35% of carers are entitled to financial state support yet don’t claim it.
  • 72% of carers suffer mental ill health due to their caring role.
  • According to Martin Lewis of MoneySavingExpert, 100,000 people are being over-charged council tax and could claim 100 of 25% of their payments back. Visit https://www.moneysavingexpert.com/reclaim/severe-mental-impairment-dementia-council-tax-rebate/ if you think this could apply to you. Councils are poorly informing people of their benefits – an MSE study found 31% of councils giving out the wrong information.

[For more information about the difficulties of caring, and to read a personal care story, do have a read of our article Challenges facing carers.]

Key strains for carers and ways to help:

  1. Physical respite (taking a break).
  2. Financial support (governmental and charitable).
  3. Emotional support (helping the carer’s wellbeing).
  4. Charities that can help.

Respite

Do not underestimate the restorative power of taking a break from your caring duties especially – but not only – if you are living with the cared-for person.

65% of older carers (aged 60-94) have long-term health problems or disabilities themselves. Scarily, one third of these carers say they have actually cancelled treatment or an operation themselves because of their caring responsibilities.

Respite allows the carer to take much-needed time off. It can prevent exhaustion and illness in the carer. It can be hard for a carer to do this. But it’s crucial that carers look after their own health too. This means occasionally prioritising your own needs and trying to avoid stress or guilt whilst doing so.

Remember, time to oneself and indeed, time off for holidays, are not luxuries but an essential component of the working year.

Your respite could be for a few hours or even a few days. The cared-for person could have a professional carer with them during your absence, or they might enter a care home temporarily.

Other possibilities to consider:

  1. Homecare with a paid carer.
  2. Short stay in a care home.
  3. Friends or family covering you for a short time.
  4. A respite holiday.
  5. ‘Sitting’ services.

NHS Advice on receiving council-aided carer respite

An assessment will then need to be conducted on your own needs as a carer, as well as the person you’re looking after.

Your local council may fund your respite, but even if you are willing to pay yourself, the assessment is useful in determining what type of care is best needed for your cared-for person. To request an assessment, contact your local council or carers’ centre.

The Assessment:

To request an assessment , you should contact your local authority – you can find yours using this website link To arrange one, contact your local services; you can find these here. https://www.nhs.uk/service-search/Local-Authority-Adult-Social-Care/LocationSearch/1918

The assessment is free for anyone over 18.

The assessment will involve someone from the council (or associated organisation) asking you questions about how you’re coping with caring (physically, mentally) and how caring has affected your work, free time and relationships. This assessment is mostly done face to face and lasts around an hour.

To prepare for the assessment, bring your:

  • NHS number.
  • GP name, address & phone number.
  • Contact details of anyone coming with you.
  • The details of the person you care for (including NHS number if possible).
  • Email address.

During, give as much detail as possible about how your life has been affected by caring. The NHS has provisions to help and can do so best if it has all the information possible.

A carer’s assessment could result in a recommendation of the following:

  • The carer to take a break from caring whilst someone else fills in.
  • Financial help with taxi fares if the carer doesn’t drive.
  • Regular gardening and housework help.
  • Household safety training.
  • Advice about benefits for carers.
  • Providing the carer with links to local support groups.

Other respite options

Day care centres:

These are often run by councils, or local charities. These offer people a chance to socialise and enjoy some activities such as teas or arts and crafts. It can give the carer a break for a day.

Charities which offer these services include Age UK and Contact the Elderly.

Paying for care

Paid carers are staff who are either live-in carers (providing 24 hour support) or regular carers (one day a week, or four times a day etc.).

How to find a carer:

  • Local council’s directory of homecare agencies (check their website).
  • NHS’s list of local homecare agencies and national homecare organisations.
  • UK Homecare Association’s list of approved homecare agencies.
  • Carers Trust’s homecare services.
  • Age UK offers some homecare services for paying clients.

Arranging a short stay in a care home:

You can use the NHS website to arrange this. Have a look at their page https://www.nhs.uk/conditions/social-care-and-support-guide/support-and-benefits-for-carers/carer-breaks-and-respite-care/

Family and friends

If you feel able to ask them, getting friends or family to stay with the cared-for person for a bit can be a wonderful (and free) way to get a break. Make sure they have all the information about the cared-for person with them before this takes place. It might be best to have a shorter, trial run of a few hours before committing to a longer break.

This will allow you, the carer and the cared-for person to predict any difficulties and create a friendly rapport.

Charities offering respite holidays

The following charities offer supportive holidays for free or at a discounted rate for carers and the cared-for. You may like to make further inquiries with them.

  1. (holidays for dementia patients and their carers).
  2. (subsidised holidays for elderly or disabled people).
  3. has planned breaks or grants and can help low income families referred by a social worker, GP, health visitor or charity.
  4. grants low income families with disabled children holidays.

Sitting services: temporary respite

These services are either free or significantly cheaper than others. Contact your local carers service,

Self-payment for respite care: you can help the person who needs care find the financial means to pay for this. This could be done through:

  1. Pensions
    1. Benefits (Attendance Allowance)
    1. Personal savings

Financial Support

There’s no way around it; caring is hugely expensive. Respite care costs £700-800 a week or up to £1500 for staying in a care home or having a live-in carer.

State support

This is worth considering as a staggering 35% of carers are not claiming for benefits they are entitled to!

If you care for at least 35 hours a week and are over the age of 16, you may well be entitled to Carer’s Allowance which would provide you with £64.60 per week. This is true even if you are not related to and/or do not live with the person you are caring for.

Visit the government website, talk to your local GP or council, or ring one of the helplines listed below to get further advice & guidance on this. Make sure you receive what the government wants to give you.

Emotional wellbeing

Relocating practical tasks can help you save time and conserve some of your much-needed energy. The internet could be a huge helping hand here. You might like to consider doing the following:

  • An online food shop – these will save your time and energy. You can even schedule repeat orders weekly or monthly.
  • Order repeat prescriptions using NHS services. Although, you will want to speak to your GP before doing so, and do not rush into using an online pharmacy service, as these could prove risky as we describe in our report here.
  • Use an app called ‘Jointly’, run by Carers UK, which helps you stay connected you’re your friends and family. https://www.jointlyapp.com/

Read our article on tech for elderly people which details more invaluable tech solutions which can ease carers’ anxieties; from automated beds to motion sensitive lighting. https://onlinefirstaid.com/technology-for-elderly/

Marie Curie Online Help

The charity Marie Curie has a wonderfully extensive page with details on wellbeing of carers to help with practical aspects of care such as how to help someone go to the toilet. It is a wealth of useful and under-shared information:

It is called ‘Becoming a carer’ and can be found here. https://www.mariecurie.org.uk/help/support/being-there/helping-someone-cope/carer-role

Stress

Being a carer is stressful: you are responsible not only for your own wellbeing, but the livelihood of another vulnerable individual. Carers UK have a page that can help you to manage your stress levels – mindfulness, meditation, respite and exercise are all recommended.

Healthy eating can also do wonders in this field. Staying in contact with friends and family, or going to local community events will give you time out and hopefully some conversational therapy time, too! Don’t be afraid to let others know of the difficulties you face as a carer. So many of us have had caring experience that others are bound to sympathise directly. Even if they haven’t, they will want to hear and relieve some of your burden for you.

Some carers feel pressure not to complain about their position, because they may feel lucky in comparison to the unwell person they are caring for. But it is absolutely normal to find caring extremely taxing. Find support in others if you can.

https://carers.org/article/reducing-stress

Counselling:

Carers should consider counselling if they feel especially stressed, depressed or overburdened. It’s a good idea to make sure your GP is aware of your caring duties and the potential for it to affect your wellbeing.

A good article to read if you are considering counselling can be found here. https://carers.org/article/counselling-carers

Free NHS health check for over-40s

Anyone over 40 should book a health assessment with their GP and are advised to do so (also for free) every five years. Carers are advised to be especially vigilant in checking their own health.

For more information, click here. https://carers.org/article/free-nhs-health-check

Free flu jabs

If you have Carer’s Allowance, or assist in caring for an elderly person, you will qualify for a free flu jab each Winter. It is strongly recommended that you take this opportunity to prevent you or the cared-for person developing an illness.

Support in case of an emergency

It is important to think about who you could contact if an emergency prevented you from caring for the looked after person.

A relative, friend or neighbour would be ideal. It would be sensible to approach this person and agree the arrangements with them.

They should:

  • Have access to the property (door keys, or knowing the code to a safe).
  • Have a relationship with the looked after person or have met them, in order to understand the care required for a short while.
  • Have a set of notes (electronic or a physical copy) about what to do. This could include medication information as well as any other essential caring information.

Charities that have specific help for carers

There are more of these than you might have thought. They offer services from phonelines (listed below), social events, financial support and more. The charity may have a local branch in your area for you to contact.

  • · Age UK

Phone lines:

Regular short breaks for constant carers

All UK employees are legally guaranteed a 20-minute break per 6 hour shift they work.

You may not see it this way, but caring is a job and therefore carers should be taking regular breaks of some form too.

Make a plan

The best way to make sure you don’t skip a break is to embed it in your schedule. Plan some time to relax and get away from your role at least once a week. Making this part of your routine will help ingrain the habit and decrease the chances that you neglect this time much needed to look after yourself.

Some suggestions are:

  • Weekly exercise class, such as gentle yoga.
  • Weekly coffee date with friends.
  • Scheduled time each evening to relax with a book or watch television.

Whatever you choose to do, providing you prioritise yourself in these moments – doing something enjoyable (a hobby or spending time with friends) or simply de-stressing alone – you will benefit from the relaxation and maintain your ability to care well.

Importance of a debrief

Professional councillors, medical staff and many others have ‘debrief’ sessions in which they can speak with colleagues about stressful conversations they have had in their jobs.

Find someone, a professional, charity volunteer or friend with whom you can share your daily experiences with someone. Even if you don’t think you need this, it is of great help to have someone who knows what you are going through and who could be of support to you if it became necessary. Speaking about your caring experiences with an exterior person can help you see your situation in a more objective manner.

If you are feeling isolated, remember help exists to be used. From face to face local guidance (both governmental and charity run) to the numerous telephone help lines, these exist to help people who care.

Ultimately:

Don’t underestimate the value of your work and please feel entitled to make use of the resources designed to help you.

In doing so, you’re looking after yourself and safeguarding the person you care for.

Written by Emma Hammett, CEO of First Aid for Life

First Aid for Life provides this information for guidance and it is not in any way a substitute for medical advice. First Aid for Life is not responsible or liable for any diagnosis made, or actions taken based on this information. It is strongly advised that you attend a First Aid course to understand what to do in a medical emergency.

Book a first aid course now – the skills you learn could equip you with the knowledge to save your child’s life! https://www.firstaidforlife.org.uk

First Aid for Life is a multi-award-winning fully regulated and Ofqual approved first aid training company. www.onlinefirstaid.com are the leading UK online first aid training providers to allow you to learn these vital skills when it suits you

Find us on Facebook, Twitter & Instagram too!

+6

Emma Hammett

Emma Hammett is a qualified nurse, author and first aid trainer with over 30 years’ healthcare and teaching experience. Emma is the Founder of three multi-award-winning businesses; First Aid for Life, Onlinefirstaid.com, First Aid for Pets and her social cause StaySafe.support. She has published multiple books and is an acknowledged first aid expert and authority on accident prevention, health and first aid. Emma writes for numerous online and print publications and regularly features in the press, on the radio and on TV. She is the first aid expert for the British Dental Journal, British Journal of School Nursing, the Mail online and Talk Radio with Eamonn Holmes. She is a member of the Guild of Health Writers and Guild of Nurses.

View Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

Sepsis: Life-Threatening Yet Difficult to Spot

This was an interesting article I found on: Talk Health Partnership

See credits below.


Sepsis is responsible for 52,000 deaths in the UK each year. A distressing 250,000 people contract the condition, which can have life changing implications; such as amputations, internal organ failure and brain damage.

Why has sepsis been in the news?

Caught early, the outcome from sepsis is good. It is the delay in treatment that can lead to spiralling of the condition resulting in multi-organ failure and potential death. The Sepsis Trust claims that improved national awareness could save as many as 14,000 lives a year.

Sadly, the early symptoms are often easy to miss and often there is a delay in Emergency Services, GPs and hospital staff recognising the condition. This delay has major implications and often the window of opportunity when antibiotics can treat the sepsis, is missed.

What is sepsis?

Sepsis arises due to the body’s abnormal response to an infection. The immune system usually works to fight any germs (such as bacteria, viruses and fungi) and infection in the body. However, for reasons that are not fully understood, sepsis results in the immune system going into overdrive and attacking health organs and other tissues.

What damage does it cause?

This inflammation causes injury to the body’s tissues and organs. The initial, localised infection spreads into the blood stream and is circulated around the body spreading and increasing the damage. This life-threatening breakdown in circulation can lead to organs all over the body being affected, including the brain, lungs, heart, skin and kidneys.

What has septicaemia got to do with it?

Sepsis is sometimes referred to as septicaemia or blood poisoning. However, septicaemia is an old, now defunct term which implied there was something we could measure in the blood which indicated sepsis – this is not the case. Sepsis is the preferred and more accurate term for the condition.

What causes sepsis?

Sepsis can be triggered by an infection in any part of the body. The most common causes of sepsis are the following;

  1. Pneumonia (or a chest infection),
  2. Urinary infections,
  3. Intra-abdominal infections (such as a burst ulcer or hole in the bowel),
  4. Skin infections (an infected cut or bite),
  5. A wound from trauma or surgery,
  6. Soft tissue infections (a leg ulcer for example).

Most cases are caused by common bacteria which wouldn’t normally induce illness. Trauma is responsible for 2-3% of reported cases.

Is it linked with meningitis?

Meningitis causes less than 1% of adult cases of sepsis. However, it is the cause in around 10% of cases in children.

Whilst most cases of meningitis are viral and therefore not life-threatening (or sepsis-inducing), a minority are bacterial infections and can lead to sepsis. Both conditions are serious and so it is important to be able to recognise common early signs and symptoms.

What is sepsis in more detail?

Sepsis is a massive reaction that damages blood flow all over the body. The immune system releases chemicals causing tiny blood vessels (capillaries) to leak fluid into the surrounding tissues. This causes inflammation which can ultimately lead to reduced blood flow to vital organs and organ damage.

The chemicals damage the capillary walls, which leak even more, causing dangerously low blood pressure. Blood clots try to repair the damage, but many break loose, causing blockages and further impairing blood flow to tissues.

If the cellular metabolism is damaged in this way and blood pressure drops to dangerously low levels, the amount of blood and oxygen reaching the body’s organs is reduced and prevents them functioning properly. This is known as septic shock.

What needs to be improved nationally?

The Royal College of Nursing held a conference in May 2019 in which they stressed the need for an urgent dissemination of a national Paediatric Early Warning Score to improve identification of the signs of sepsis in children. Currently, between 1000 and 4000 children die each year from the condition. An adult symptoms checklist is already used by the NHS, but trusts use individual systems for children which results in an inconsistent and unreliable approach

A symptoms checklist for adults already exists but NHS trusts often use their own systems for children. The children’s checklist would help medics spot when a youngster is deteriorating through:

  1. Checking temperature,
  2. Heart rate,
  3. Respiration rate,
  4. Other signs, such as urination, skin colour and rash.

The Sepsis Trust claim that patients presenting early being around half as likely to die than those presenting late. This emphasises the importance of awareness.

The condition deteriorates in children far quicker, and therefore it is particularly important for those in childcaring positions to be fully informed of the signs and symptoms.

What would I feel like if I had sepsis?

  1. Flu-like symptoms are common,
  2. Chest infection or gastroenteritis type symptoms,
  3. Feverish and shivering – although sepsis can cause an abnormally high or low temperature.
  4. Rapid heartbeat,
  5. Quick breathing
  6. Feeling extremely unwell.

In more extreme cases, you may experience:

  • Dizziness,
  • Feel faint,
  • Disorientation and confusion,
  • Nausea and vomiting,
  • Diarrhoea,
  • Cold, clammy, pale or mottled skin.

In children:

  1. Fast breathing,
  2. Lethargic behaviour and fatigue,
  3. Mottled, bluish or pale skin,
  4. Fits or convulsions,
  5. Failure to pass urine,
  6. A rash that doesn’t fade when pressed – NEVER wait for a rash, it is often a very late sign and may not show at all.

Any baby or child under 5 years old who is not feeding, vomiting repeatedly or hasn’t had a wee or wet nappy for 12 hours, might have sepsis.

What do I need to remember?

If you think you have these symptoms, or feel that something isn’t right, seek urgent medical advice (e.g. call NHS 111 or go into hospital). It is vital that you trust your instincts and get help to fight the infection as fast as possible.

Written by Emma Hammett for First Aid for Life

Award-winning first aid training tailored to your needs – Please visit our site and learn more about our practical and online courses. It is vital to keep your skills current and refreshed.

It is strongly advised that you attend a fully regulated Practical or Online First Aid course to understand what to do in a medical emergency. Please visit https://firstaidforlife.org.uk or call 0208 675 4036 for more information about our courses.

First Aid for Life is a multi-award-winning, fully regulated first aid training provider. Our trainers are highly experienced medical, health and emergency services professionals who will tailor the training to your needs. Courses for groups or individuals at our venue or yours.

First Aid for life provides this information for guidance and it is not in any way a substitute for medical advice. First Aid for Life is not responsible or liable for any diagnosis made, or actions taken based on this information.

+4

Emma Hammett

Emma Hammett is a qualified nurse, author and first aid trainer with over 30 years’ healthcare and teaching experience. Emma is the Founder of three multi-award-winning businesses; First Aid for Life, Onlinefirstaid.com, First Aid for Pets and her social cause StaySafe.support. She has published multiple books and is an acknowledged first aid expert and authority on accident prevention, health and first aid. Emma writes for numerous online and print publications and regularly features in the press, on the radio and on TV. She is the first aid expert for the British Dental Journal, British Journal of School Nursing, the Mail online and Talk Radio with Eamonn Holmes. She is a member of the Guild of Health Writers and Guild of Nurses.

View Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

UK Mental Health Research Funding: Where next?

This was an interesting article I found on: M. Q. Mental Health

See credits below.


MQ has just published the second ‘UK Mental Health Research Funding’ report, which sets out the latest data on exactly how much money has been invested in mental health research – and where it’s spent.

We asked some of the leading voices in UK mental health research one question: what needs to happen next?

Professor Chris Whitty, NIHR lead and Chief Scientific Advisor to the Department of Health and Social Care:

"As leader of the NIHR – the nation’s largest funder of mental health research – I welcome MQ's call for broad, sustained investment. NIHR is building partnerships across government, the funding community, researchers and the voluntary sector, in order to deliver the Government’s ambition of parity between mental and physical health."

Dr Andrew Welchman, Head of Neuroscience & Mental Health at Wellcome Trust:

“As this report shows, too many people are still left without effective treatments and we need to invest to meet this challenge. The increased focus on mental health by the public and politicians is great progress, but talking alone is not enough. We need to develop and improve treatments and get these to the people who need them.

“Science is essential to advancing mental health, which is why Wellcome is committing £200 million for mental health research over the next five years to drive the field forward. To take on this huge challenge, we need broad expertise, with researchers from different backgrounds and experiences, and different countries, working alongside individual’s with personal experience, governments, businesses and wider society.”

Dr Vanessa Pinfold, Chair of the Alliance of Mental Health Research funders and co-founder of the McPin Foundation:

“Tracking mental health funding levels across institutions and different topic areas is vital to ensure the scarce resources available to mental health research can be appropriately targeted to make the biggest difference in people’s lives. I welcome a future vision focused on a multi-disciplinary approach to mental health research, and in particular the inclusion of lived experience expertise.”

Dr Sophie Dix, Director of Research at MQ:

“We need research to work for people living with a mental illness. This report shows there’s a lot of work to be done to see real progress. We need more funding, of course, but we also need funding to be used more effectively.

"The need for charities like MQ to not only raise important funds, but also work collaboratively is clear. It’s vital we come together and ensure that all research being funded has a pathway to reach those affected. Research can transform what it means to experience mental illness, starting now and for every generation to come. It’s time to make this happen.”

Dr Sarah Carr, Senior Fellow in Mental Health Policy, Institute for Mental Health, University of Birmingham:

"I think that one of the issues is that there isn't a coherent narrative on mental health research for the public, which conveys biopsychosocial understandings rather than disease models. The distribution of what little funding there is demonstrates this. Mental health is complex and the research needs to be multi-facted. We need to invest in research into social factors such as poverty, trauma and discrimination, finding out how we live with mental distress and harnessing experiential knowledge."

Professor Louise Arseneault, ESRC Mental Health Leadership Fellow:

“Greater mental health awareness may generate greater public and philanthropic donations, but we need a framework around whether this is on an institutional, charity or funder level.

"We can support a loud and bold mental health awareness campaign that not only appeals to the general population but goes deep to reach the most disadvantaged and vulnerable groups of people who need knowledge and education about mental health. It is also our responsibility, as a community of mental health researchers, to propose the best and the most innovative research projects that are at the forefront of science.”

André Tomlin, researcher and founder of the Mental Elf:

“We need more research funding, and we need it targeted in areas where there is the most inequality, discrimination and stigma. But let’s not ignore a more fundamental problem: it still takes 17 years for new research to reach frontline practice. Disseminating and implementing new evidence in practice must be our real priority if we are to see genuine improvements in patient care.”

Click here to read the full UK Mental Health Research Funding 2014-2017 report, or the methodology paper in The Lancet Psychiatry.

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

Dr Shahzad Malik becomes MQ’s new Chair

This was an interesting article I found on: M. Q. Mental Health

See credits below.


MQ has today announced that Dr Shahzad Malik has become the organisation’s new Chair. He takes over from Sir Philip Campbell, who stepped down as Chair last month, while remaining on the Board until the end of the year.

Shahzad is General Partner of Advent Life Sciences, one of Europe's most established venture and growth capital firms investing in world-class life sciences businesses.

He has been an MQ Trustee since 2017, bringing significant business, scientific and clinical expertise to the Board. He has an MA in Physiological Sciences from Oxford University and an MD from Cambridge University. Before working at Advent Life Sciences, he specialised in interventional cardiology while at the same time pursuing research interests in heart muscle disorders, both in the clinic and basic science laboratory.

Commenting, Shahzad said: “It is an honour to become Chair of this incredible charity and take over from Sir Philip Campbell. In his work with MQ, Phil has shown true leadership and given his own considerable expertise and time to help drive MQ forward.

Through investing in impactful science, providing leadership, and building unprecedented public support, MQ is truly transforming mental health through research. As Chair, I look forward to working with and supporting the MQ team to further deliver this unique and vital mission.”

Sir Philip Campbell stepped down as Chair in April. Having been a Founding Trustee when MQ was set up in 2013, he has served as Chair since 2015.

In 2018, Sir Philip was appointed Editor-in-Chief of the publisher Springer Nature, having been Editor-in-Chief of Nature for over 20 years. In his new position he is specifically tasked with working internationally to lead several of Springer Nature’s global challenge initiatives. He is stepping down as MQ Chair as he pursues this new role, but will remain an active member of the Board until the end of the year.

Sir Philip said: “I have been privileged to be a Board member of MQ since its very beginnings. As Chair since 2015, I have been inspired by the significant achievements of our researchers and staff, by the calibre and commitment of my fellow trustees, and by the very substantial donor support for our programmes in adolescence research, data platforms and the MQ Fellows.

As all of that work and support continues, and as we undertake the appointment of our next CEO and explore new partnerships, now is also the right time for fresh leadership of the trustees. I gave the Board notice of my intention to stand down earlier this year. I will be delighted to continue to serve on the Board under my successor Shahzad Malik. I will also be glad to continue to be a member of MQ’s Research Committee after I cease being a trustee at the end of this year.”

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

How to Tell the Difference Between Bipolar and Borderline Personality

This is an interesting article I found on: www.goodtherapy.org

See credits below.


Impulsivity, mood swings, irritability, high and low periods, patterns of troubled relationships—these symptoms often indicate bipolar, but they can just as easily appear in people who have borderline personality (BPD).

Neither condition is uncommon. Approximately 2.6% of adults in the United States live with bipolar. Estimates for BPD vary, but it’s believed somewhere between 1.6% and 5.9% of adults in the U.S. live with this condition. Many people have a dual diagnosis, or both conditions.

The resemblance between the traits characterizing each issue and the possibility of co-occurrence has led some professionals to question whether BPD is a subtype or variation of bipolar. The general consensus among mental health experts, however, is that while these conditions often present with similar features, they are two separate mental health issues that can usually be distinguished in a few key ways.

It’s during periods of mania that bipolar may be most suggestive of BPD, as manic episodes often involve thrill-seeking, impulsive, or aggressive behavior.

Bipolar vs. Borderline Personality

A mood disorder, bipolar is primarily characterized by shifts between high-energy (manic) states and low-energy (depressive) states. Bipolar-related mood changes can range from mild to extreme, and they’re typically accompanied by changes in a person’s energy and activity.

Not every person who has bipolar will experience a classic manic episode. These episodes generally last several days and frequently involve increased activity and productivity in schoolwork, work-related tasks, or creative pursuits. Feeling very energized or charged, with little or no need to sleep, is common.

People living with bipolar II experience milder manic periods known as hypomania. Cyclothymia, a subtype of bipolar, involves hypomanic and depressive periods that don’t meet typical bipolar criteria. But mania is a symptom specifically linked to bipolar, so having even one manic episode indicates bipolar in most cases.

It’s during periods of mania that bipolar may be most suggestive of BPD, as manic episodes often involve thrill-seeking, impulsive, or aggressive behavior. Impulsive actions might include risky sex, excessive spending, or substance abuse, along with other behavior that isn’t typical. Rapid cycling bipolar may particularly resemble BPD, as mood fluctuations happen more frequently than with typical bipolar.

Frequent manic episodes could also contribute to relationship difficulties, since the way a person behaves during a manic episode could have a negative impact on the people close to them. For example, during a manic episode, a person in a monogamous relationship may cheat on their partner or decide to redo all of their home furnishings and max out multiple credit cards in order to purchase new interior decorations. A person who uses drugs during a manic episode could face legal consequences, especially if their actions while under the influence of drugs cause harm to others.

But with BPD, particularly untreated BPD, emotional shifts tend to be sudden and happen frequently. BPD is a personality disorder, so the associated traits don’t simply relate to mood changes, they’re persistent behavior patterns. Extreme, all-or-nothing thinking patterns also help characterize this condition. For example, a person with BPD who experiences mild criticism at work may become very upset and distressed. They may feel they’ve failed and fear they’ll lose their job.

Another characteristic of BPD is difficulty interpreting emotions. People often view neutral or other expressions as negative, and this misinterpretation could lead to conflict or strained personal relationships.

Similarly, a minor disagreement with a partner could lead someone to believe they’re unlovable and the relationship is over. They might end the relationship first, fearing rejection. Relationship conflict can also trigger devaluation of a partner who was previously idealized, depending on the circumstances. With devaluation, feelings of anger, disdain, and contempt may abruptly replace feelings of love and happiness in the relationship.

Lifetime suicide risk is high with either bipolar or BPD, while recurring non-suicidal self-harming behaviors as well as multiple suicide attempts are common with BPD. Cutting and other self-harm doesn’t necessarily indicate suicidal intent. Research indicates many people with BPD self-harm as a way of coping or as a way of feeling something during a period of dissociation.

How Do Treatment Approaches Differ?

These two conditions have separate underlying causes, though people with a family history of either bipolar or BPD have a higher risk for that condition.

The causes of BPD aren’t fully known, but it’s believed to develop from a combination of factors. A tendency to experience extreme emotionality, which can also run in families, is believed to contribute, especially in people who’ve experienced abuse, trauma, and neglect. Brain chemistry is a significant contributing factor to bipolar, though environmental factors can also increase risk.

Correct diagnosis is important, because treatment approaches vary depending on the condition. It’s important to understand that therapy alone typically can’t treat mania in people living with bipolar. It may also not be enough to treat severe depression in some people.

Therapy can help address some symptoms and challenges of living with bipolar, but in most cases people with typical bipolar will need medication to help stabilize mood shifts. Untreated mania and depression can have serious emotional and even physical health consequences, so it’s important to seek, and continue with, treatment.

Mood stabilizers such as lithium won’t help BPD symptoms. In some cases, bipolar treatment might even make certain symptoms worse. There’s no medication that specifically treats BPD. The typical treatment is dialectical behavior therapy, though other therapy approaches such as schema therapy can also have significant benefit.

Can Bipolar and Borderline Personality Co-occur?

A person experiencing symptoms of both bipolar and borderline personality may have both conditions.

A person experiencing symptoms of both bipolar and borderline personality may have both conditions. This isn’t uncommon, in fact. A 2013 review of multiple studies on the two conditions found that around 10% of people diagnosed with borderline personality also had bipolar I, while about 10% had bipolar II as well as BPD.

Living with untreated borderline personality and bipolar can cause significant distress, in part because the two conditions may play off each other.

  • Feelings of emptiness or failure may be even worse during a bipolar depressive period, causing emotional turmoil or disconnect, both of which may increase risk for self-harming behavior or suicide.
  • A person struggling with trust or abandonment issues in their relationship could have an even harder time maintaining a healthy relationship during a low mood state.
  • A period of mania may be more likely to trigger risky or impulsive behavior in a person who feels distressed or disconnected from their sense of self and wants to feel something.
  • Substance abuse isn’t uncommon with BPD or bipolar, and alcohol and drugs can often trigger mania.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) recommends mental health professionals avoid diagnosing personality disorders during untreated mood episodes. Taking a detailed mental health history that looks back at patterns and symptoms over a longer period of time can help differentiate the two conditions.

Between manic and depressive episodes, people with bipolar generally experience fairly normal moods. Months or even years could pass between high and low periods, especially when treatment is effective at managing symptoms. So once a mood episode has stabilized, diagnosis may be somewhat clearer. When a manic or depressive mood seems to respond to treatment but symptoms of emotional dysregulation persist, a dual diagnosis is likely.

Treatment for Co-occurring Bipolar and Borderline Personality

Living with co-occurring BPD and bipolar may be more challenging than having either condition alone, especially if it takes time to get an accurate diagnosis. Bipolar-related mood swings, when combined with more frequent and rapid changes in emotional state, can make daily life difficult and negatively affect work, school, and personal life. People living with bipolar and BPD may feel even more unstable or unable to control what’s happening around them than those living with only one of these conditions.

While treatment such as therapy can be very helpful for reducing symptoms and improving quality of life, the recommended treatments for each condition differ. This makes an accurate diagnosis essential for successful treatment.

For bipolar, therapy may involve learning to recognize mood triggers, developing ways to cope with bipolar symptoms, and working to reduce the effects symptoms have on daily life. The combination of mood stabilizing medication and dialectical behavior therapy may be recommended for people with both bipolar and BPD, since DBT is generally the ideal approach to therapy for BPD. This therapy involves developing the skills to manage and cope with difficult emotions and practicing positive ways of relating to others.

For people experiencing BPD-related distress during a manic or depressive episode, mood stabilization is an important first step. Research suggests BPD symptoms may improve slightly once mood has stabilized, which can increase the chance of successful treatment. It’s also essential to talk about suicidal thoughts or self-harm, since these may be more likely in people with both conditions than people who only have bipolar.

Psychotic symptoms such as hallucinations can also occur during a manic episode, and these can be dangerous. They’re not as common with BPD, but they do occur, so it’s important to discuss any hallucinations, delusions, or magical thinking when a person presents with symptoms of both conditions.

Finding a Therapist for Bipolar or Borderline Personality

For some mental health concerns, diagnosis may not significantly impact treatment since symptoms can still be addressed in therapy. But when bipolar and BPD, which sometimes present similarly, are misdiagnosed for each other, treatment may be less effective. Symptoms of both conditions can further complicate diagnosis. Some mental health professionals may fail to recognize the presence of both issues, particularly if they’re less experienced with the differences between the two or unaware bipolar and BPD often occur together.

When seeking a diagnosis or working to address symptoms of both bipolar and BPD, it’s recommended to seek support from a therapist who has experiencing helping people with both conditions. While other trained, empathic therapists can certainly provide compassionate care, a therapist who specializes in working with people living with these conditions may offer support that’s designed to address specific symptoms of these conditions. This may be especially important when beginning therapy for the first time.

References:

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
  2. Bipolar disorder. (2017). National Alliance on Mental Illness. Retrieved from https://www.nami.org/learn-more/mental-health-conditions/bipolar-disorder
  3. Bipolar disorder. (2018). National Institute of Mental Health. Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/publications/bipolar-disorder/index.shtml
  4. Borderline personality disorder. (2017). National Alliance on Mental Illness. Retrieved from https://www.nami.org/Learn-More/Mental-Health-Conditions/Borderline-Personality-Disorder
  5. Borderline personality disorder. (2017). National Institute of Mental Health. Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/borderline-personality-disorder/index.shtml
  6. Fenske, S., Lis, S., Liebke, L., Niedtfeld, I., Kirsch, P., & Mier, D. (2015, June 26). Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: Effects of emotional information on negative bias. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 2, 10. doi: 10.1186/s40479-015-0031-z
  7. Ghaemi, S. N., Dalley, S., Catania, C., & Barroilhet, S. (2014). Bipolar or borderline: A clinical overview. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 130(2), 99-108. doi: 10.1111/acps.12257
  8. Kvarnstrom, E. (2017, October 5). Borderline personality disorder misdiagnosed as bipolar disorder: Differences and treatment. Retrieved from https://www.bridgestorecovery.com/blog/borderline-personality-disorder-misdiagnosed-as-bipolar-disorder-differences-and-treatment
  9. Linehan, M. M., Korslund, K. E., & Harned, M. S. (2015). Dialectical behavior therapy for high suicide risk in individuals with borderline personality disorder: A randomized clinical trial and component analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(5), 475-482. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3039
  10. Paris, J. (2004). Borderline or bipolar? Distinguishing borderline personality disorder from bipolar spectrum disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12(3), 140-145. doi: 10.1080/10673220490472373
  11. Zimmerman, M., & Morgan, T. A. (2013). The relationship between borderline personality disorder and bipolar disorder. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15(2), 155-169. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3811087

© Copyright 2019 GoodTherapy.org. All rights reserved.

The preceding article was solely written by the author named above. Any views and opinions expressed are not necessarily shared by GoodTherapy.org. Questions or concerns about the preceding article can be directed to the author or posted as a comment below.

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!

The Untold Impact of Mother-Son Incest

This is an interesting article I found on: www.goodtherapy.org

See credits below.


This topic likely comes as a surprise to many. Just the idea of abuse of this nature, between a mother and her son, is shocking to most. The idea of mother-son incest is so far out of the realm of what we as a culture understand about mothers and women that even its victims rarely seek help.

As a society, our views of mothers as nurturers who would never willingly hurt their children may be so ingrained in our psyche that even trained psychologists can be uncomfortable entertaining the idea that sexual abuse can happen between a mother and her son (1).

Why the Silence?

Incest (sexual relationships between family members) is taboo and can bring a strong sense of guilt and shame to its victims (2). While the idea that fathers sexually abuse their children is disturbing, it is accepted as something that can (and does) happen. It is well documented and studied.

Although the idea that some fathers can be sexual predators towards their own family is accepted, the parallel idea, that mothers can be sexual predators towards their own children, has not been widely accepted. We live in a culture that tends to idolize motherhood. Mothers sacrifice so much to give us everything we need. In our society, speaking against a mother is almost sacrilegious. Unfortunately, the perception of a male monopoly on perpetrating incest has led to the creation of damaging myths that silence the male victim.

Reporting incest and seeking professional help may be both shameful and difficult in any situation, but it can be even more difficult in the case of a mother. Often, the reaction will be complete rejection or disbelief. Unfortunately, the perception of a male monopoly on perpetrating incest has led to the creation of damaging myths that silence the male victim.

Males and Sexual Abuse: The Myths

Researcher Lucetta Thomas has identified persistent and damaging myths in regard to male sexual victimization. These myths not only exist in the minds of boys and men who themselves are victims—they are also prevalent in the attitudes and perception of social workers, law enforcement, and even psychologists or counselors (3). Myths around males and sexual abuse include the following:

  • Boys and men can’t be sexually victimized; they must have consented.
  • Mothers do not do this; she must have been overly affectionate.
  • If the boy experiences sexual arousal or pleasure during the abuse, he enjoyed it, and it was not abuse, because he participated.
  • Boys are less traumatized by sexual abuse than girls, and this is because boys are more sex-focused in general.
  • The mother or son must have mental health issues.

Prevalence and Long-Term Outcomes of Mother-Son Abuse

Due to the refusal of boys and men to seek help or press charges against mothers who abuse them, it is nearly impossible to determine the prevalence of sexual abuse committed by mothers. However, a few studies offer surprising results and indicate the problem is more widespread than most people would assume.

For example, one study that conducted in-depth interviews of seven men and seven women who reported sexual abuse by a female perpetrator, most of whom experienced severe sexual abuse by their mothers, found a range of long-term damaging effects. Victims reported and/or experienced depression, difficulties with substance abuse, self-injury, increased suicide rate, rage, strained relationships with women, identity issues, and discomfort with sex (4).

Another study conducted in 2002 found that 17 of 67 men who endured sexual abuse during childhood reported mother-son incest (5). The study found in comparison to the other men in the study, the men who were abused by their mothers experienced more symptoms of trauma. Further, about half of the men abused by their mothers had mixed feelings regarding the abuse, and those with mixed feelings had more adjustment problems compared to men who had purely negative feelings toward the abuse (5).

Lucetta Thomas reported that after her story of mother-son sexual abuse aired on ABC 80, males accessed the online survey over the next two days to report maternal abuse and requested to be interviewed. It must be understood that this type of abuse is possible, does happen, and can do extraordinary damage to its victims.

When we examine outcomes of victims of any type of incest, we find this type of abuse is related to issues around relational trauma and betrayal trauma. Abuse by a trusted family member leads to a significant loss of trust and changes in beliefs around the self and safety in relationships (2). Understandably, when the perpetrator is a mother, the trauma is likely to carry a particularly high level of damage, especially in light of the cultural perceptions of mothers as nurturers. Furthermore, the implications of reporting abuse of this nature can be catastrophic for the victim, the mother, and the entire family. In many cases, this leaves the victim feeling as if he has no choice but to deal with the trauma in silence.

What Professionals Need to Know

Professionals, particularly those working with sexual abuse cases, need to examine their own perceptions around women as potential abusers. It must be understood that this type of abuse is possible, does happen, and can do extraordinary damage to its victims. In general, many people have been under the impression that a woman cannot really harm another person sexually. This is not the case. As new research surfaces, we are finding that sexual abuse from mother to son can bring lasting trauma and long-term mental health effects (4).

Further, men and boys are much less likely to report sexual abuse (6). Researchers have put forth the possibility that attitudes and beliefs among mental health professionals in myths regarding the male as an unlikely victim do not create conditions that encourage men or boys to talk about sexual abuse. Professionals need to be aware of the reality of mother-son sexual abuse as well as the existence of the myths surrounding the male as unlikely to be vulnerable to sexual abuse and especially unlikely to be the victim of abuse by his own mother.

If you are a victim of any type of sexual abuse or assault, reach out to a therapist. There is no need to suffer in silence when help is available. If you are a victim of mother-son incest, clearly articulate your experiences to your therapist. The shame is not yours.

References:

  1. Osborne, T. (2015, August 7). New research sheds light on sex abuse committed by mothers against their sons. ABC News. Retrieved from https://www.abc.net.au/news/2015-08-08/new-research-mothers-who-sexually-abuse-their-sons/6679102
  2. Kluft, R. P. (2011, January 12). Ramifications of incest. Psychiatric Times, 27(12). Retrieved from https://www.psychiatrictimes.com/sexual-offenses/ramifications-incest
  3. Friedersdorf, C. (2016, November 28). The understudied female sexual predator. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/11/the-understudied-female-sexual-predator/503492
  4. Denov, M. S. (2004, October 1). The long-term effects of child sexual abuse by female perpetrators: A qualitative study of male and female victims. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19(10), 1,137-1,156. doi: 10.1177/0886260504269093
  5. Kelly, R. J., Wood, J. J., Gonzalez, L. S., MacDonald, V., & Waterman, J. (2002). Effects of mother-son incest and positive perceptions of sexual abuse experiences on the psychosocial adjustment of clinic-referred men. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26(4), 425-441. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12092807
  6. Holmes, G. R., Offen, L., & Waller, G. (1997). See no evil, hear no evil, speak no evil: Why do relatively few male victims of childhood sexual abuse receive help for abuse-related issues in adulthood?. Clinical Psychology Review, 17(1), 69-88. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9125368

© Copyright 2019 GoodTherapy.org. All rights reserved. Permission to publish granted by Fabiana Franco, PhD, therapist in New York City, New York

The preceding article was solely written by the author named above. Any views and opinions expressed are not necessarily shared by GoodTherapy.org. Questions or concerns about the preceding article can be directed to the author or posted as a comment below.

Original Article

Hope you enjoyed reading this article.
Please feel free to share!